The Neanderthal Legacy by Paul Mellars

Stevie Argyll said:
Thanks for the clarification. Have you seen or do you have a link to the original write up that these two articles are based on? I am wondering how large the sample size that the researchers are basing their conclusions on is.

Laura quoted the paper on the previous page:

We performed this test using eight present-day humans: two European Americans, two East Asians, and four West Africans, for whom sequences have been generated... We find that the Neandertals are equally close to Europeans and East Asians.... However, the Neandertals are significantly closer to non-Africans than to Africans.

Not a great sample, to say the least. And at the beginning, they say they are assuming an "average divergence", which, if I understand the jargon correctly (high chance I don't), means they just averaged the results for everyone they tested.
 
Well.... that was intense. I'm going to re-read Bradley's book. Maybe we ought to run that article on sott?
 
Laura said:
If anybody wants a copy of this impossible to get book, I have a scan of it.

If not trouble, I would like to read it. Thanks Laura. :)

Great thematic :D


Found nice artivcle about neanderthals and canibalism and cannibalism and transmissible spongiform encephalopathies:

First about Neanderthal and Canibalism;

Neanderthals Were Cannibals, Bones Show

Elizabeth Culotta

Neanderthals were skilled hunters, working together to fell deer, goats, and perhaps even woolly rhinos with wooden spears. After the kill, they expertly butchered the carcasses, slicing meat and tendons from bone with stone tools and bashing open long bones to get at the fatty marrow inside. Now, on page 128, a French and American team reports that 100,000-year-old Neanderthals at the French cave of Moula-Guercy performed precisely the same kinds of butchery on some of their own kind.

Marks on the bones clearly reveal that these early humans filleted the chewing muscle from the heads of two young Neanderthals, sliced out the tongue of at least one, and smashed the leg bone of a large adult to get at the marrow. The bone fragments were apparently then dumped amid the remains of deer and other butchered mammals. "Human and mammal remains were treated very similarly," says first author Alban Defleur of the Université du Mediterrané at Marseilles. "We can safely infer that both species were exploited for a culinary goal."

Tantalizing hints of cannibalism have been spotted at other Neanderthal sites for decades, but this is far and away the best documented case, say other researchers, who praise the team's careful comparison of breakage and cut marks in deer and human bones. "Quite convincing," says anthropologist Fred H. Smith of Northern Illinois University in De Kalb, noting that there's little sign of gnawing or other indications that carnivores rather than people mauled the bones. "And the documented cut marks seal the deal."

Smith and a few others say that without an eyewitness, we may never know exactly why Neanderthals handled corpses so seemingly brutally. But most paleoanthropologists are unfazed by the idea of early humans eating each other. As Milford Wolpoff of the University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, puts it, "Why should modern humans be the only violent ones?"

Defleur began to zero in on cannibalism after he saw cut marks on human bones from a test pit sunk into the cave at Moula-Guercy, a site that had previously yielded stone tools characteristic of the Neanderthals' Mousterian culture. He teamed up with paleoanthropologist Tim White of the University of California, Berkeley, to rigorously compare the pattern of marks on the human bones with those on bones from red deer, presumably hunted for meat, at the same site.

The bones--78 pieces identified as belonging to at least six humans and almost 400 fragments attributed to other mammals--were scattered over 20 square meters. All the braincases and long bones of both deer and humans were smashed open, presumably to allow brains and marrow to be extracted. "In both taxa, marrow bones were systematically broken, and bones without marrow were not damaged," says Defleur.

Analysis of three pieces of a large thigh bone showed how, after its muscles were sliced away, it was set on an anvil stone and hit repeatedly with another stone. Telltale striations mark the bone's outer surface on the anvil side, directly opposite "percussion pits" made by the hammerstone. Cut marks on the clavicle also show where the Neanderthals disarticulated the arm at the shoulder. Others reveal where they cut out tongue and jaw muscles, severed the Achilles' tendon, and sliced other tendons below the toes and at the elbow. The bones bear few signs of burning or roasting, says White, suggesting that even though the Neanderthals had fire, they ate this flesh raw or hacked it off the bone before cooking. "The circumstantial forensic evidence [of cannibalism] is excellent. No mortuary practice has ever been shown to leave these patterns on the resulting osteological assemblages," he says.

on: http://cas.bellarmine.edu/tietjen/Human%20Nature%20S%201999/neanderthals_were_cannibals.htm

or:

The slashed and butchered bones of at least eight Neanderthal people who lived 43,000 years ago were excavated from a cave called El Sidron in the Asturias region of Spain by a research team led by Antonio Rosas, a paleoanthropologist.

The remains of four young adults, two teenagers, one youngster and an infant all bore deliberate cut marks made by the crude stone tools of the era, including saw-toothed knives, skin scrapers and a single hand ax, report Rosas and his colleagues.

There is also evidence that some of the skulls of the eight Neanderthals were skinned, their leg joints were dismembered, and other long bones were broken -- presumably to extract the fat and protein from the rich marrow, Rosas said.

Rosas' team has been excavating the huge cave near the town of Oviedo for nearly seven years and has discovered more than 1,300 hominid bones and scraps of bone there. But what struck Rosas most sharply was that the cave held no remains of animals that might have preyed on Neanderthals; the team found only seven animal bones there -- from one large browsing elk and a fox. He also pointed out there were no tooth marks on the Neanderthal bones that could have been made by a beast of prey.

A report on the new discoveries at the El Sidron cave was published online last week in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

on: http://articles.sfgate.com/2006-12-11/news/17323158_1_neanderthals-cave-bones

Sci/Tech

Neanderthals were cannibals

This Neanderthal thigh bone was smashed open for its marrow

Gory evidence uncovered in France reveals that the early humans in the region ate one another.

Cheek muscles from children were filleted out, tendons were sliced and skulls were cracked to remove brains.


[ image: Sharp butchering marks made by flint tools]
Sharp butchering marks made by flint tools
Commenting on the research published in the journal Science, anthropologist Juan Luis Arsuaga said: "To me this is, paradoxically, a very human behaviour that indicates a human mind. Only humans practice systematic cannibalism - this is the dark side of the human coin."

Excavations at the cave at Moula-Guercy, Ardeche, yielded 78 Neanderthal bones, from at least six individuals who lived 100,000 years ago. Remnants of two adults, two 15 or 16 year-olds, and two six or seven year-olds were dug up as well as nearly 400 pieces of animal bone.



Dr Tim White: We are quite convinced by the evidence
Careful study of tool marks and fractures on the remains shows that these Neanderthals were master butchers.

"If we conclude that the animal remains are the leftovers from a meal, we're obliged to expand that conclusion to include humans," said the research team leader Alban Defleur, at the University of the Mediterranean Marseille.


[ image: Skull fragments: hammered open to remove brains]
Skull fragments: hammered open to remove brains
All the skulls and limb bones were broken apart, presumably to remove brain or marrow. Only the hand and foot bones remained intact, which contain no marrow. Arm and leg tendons were cut, a necessary action if a limb is to be removed. Other cuts show that the thigh muscles were removed, and in at least one case the tongue was cut out.

There have been hints of Neanderthal cannibalism at other sites before but this is the by far the clearest evidence and the first in Europe.

No signs of gnawing were found on the bones, ruling out the possibility that the Neanderthals were eaten by wild animals. There were no signs of charring either suggesting the flesh was either eaten raw or cooked off the bone.

on: http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/science/nature/462048.stm

But most interesting articles about Neanderthal are about posible conection between Neandertals, cannibalism and transmissible spongiform encephalopathies:



Did Neandertals die off because of cannibalism and transmissible spongiform encephalopathies?

So there’s this hypotheses that Neandertal extinction was due to cannibalism. This is an alternative but complementary hypothesis to the climate change one. In an upcoming paper in Medical Hypotheses, Simon Underdown investigates this hypotheses by looking at Transmissible Spongiform Encephalopathies (TSEs). The paper is titled, “A potential role for Transmissible Spongiform Encephalopathies in Neanderthal extinction.”

TSEs are also known as prion diseases, a communicable disease where the infectious agent is a malformed protein that replicates by imprinting and transforming other proteins. Most TSEs manifest in the host’s neurological tissues because he or she ate infected nervous tissue. Ultimately, the host’s tissues degenerate and lead to serious problems, most often death. In anthropology, one form of TSEs has been well documented, the spread and eradication of kuru in the Fore from Papua New Guinea.


Before I read the abstract, I assumed Simon looked at Neandertal fossils for paleopathological evidence of spongiform encephalopathies…. at least to some degree. However, the abstract doesn’t definitively indicate whether or not Simon looked into the Neandertal fossil record. Instead, the abstract tells us,

“A modern human hunter-gatherer proxy has been developed and applied as a hypothetical model to the Neanderthals. This hypothesis suggests that the impact of TSEs on the Neanderthals could have been dramatic and have played a large part in contributing to the processes of Neanderthal extinction.”

For those that are curious if there’s evidence of Neandertal cannibalism, you’re in luck. Neandertal cannibalism has been previously documented from remains from sites like Moula-Guercy caves in Ardèche, France, and El Sidrón, Asturias, Spain, so it is totally probable that some got prion diseases. But they were eating each other for 100,000 or so years before the faded out of the picture. You’d think that if they died of prion dieases it wouldn’t take so damn long. Anyways, I just really wonder how Simon went about figuring out if Neandertals really got TSEs.

UNDERDOWN, S. (2008). A potential role for Transmissible Spongiform Encephalopathies in Neanderthal extinction. Medical Hypotheses DOI: 10.1016/j.mehy.2007.12.014

on: http://anthropology.net/2008/02/29/did-neandertal-die-off-because-of-cannibalism-and-transmissible-spongiform-encephalopathies/

or:

Mad Neanderthal Disease?

The impact of Transmissible Spongiform Encephalopathies (TSE) on Neanderthals is the topic of a short paper by Simon Underdown (2008), in press in Medical Hypotheses. You might be familiar with TSEs already: a few years back, the so-called “Mad Cow Disease” scare was caused by Bovine Spongiform Encephalopathy.

Getting back to the study at hand, the gist of the paper is this:

1) Neanderthals are known to have practiced cannibalism.

2) Cannibalism is known to result in TSEs in certain cases.

3) In at least one Homo sapiens hunter-gatherer group (in this case the Fore of New Guinea), the relationship between cannibalism and some kind of fatal degenerative neurological disease has been credibly established (the 'Kuru' epidemic'; Farquhar and Gadjusek 1981).

4) Therefore, it is possible that TSEs transmitted through cannibalism and handling dead Neanderthal tissue contributed to the ‘extinction’ Neanderthals, to wit: “… a silent killer in the form of TSEs could have massively weakened the Neanderthal’s ability to compete both within a highly changeable environment and latterly against a highly adaptable new arrival in the form of Homo sapiens” (Underdown in press: p.3).


The paper itself does a much better job of contextualizing this conclusion than headlines related to the diffusion of this study such as “Cannibalism wiped out Neanderthals” which is overly sensationalistic on top of fostering the curious image of Neanderthals eating one another into oblivion (which would certainly put a new spin on the old ‘competition with moderns over animal resources’!).

And, while it is nearly impossible to conclusively disprove many paleoanthropological hypotheses, it is certainly possible to evaluate their plausibility, and the case for TSEs as a, errr, vector in Neanderthal extinction is improbable. First, the use of the Fore as the single analog is problematic on several levels: they are horticulturalists, a poor analog for highly mobile hunter-gatherers; the kuru epidemic on which Underdown’s scenario depends unfolded over a span of only decades whereas he postulates that TSEs would’ve plagued Neanderthals for millennia; and, importantly, the Fore live at population densities (about 21/km2), orders of magnitude greater than those likely to have characterized, and certainly those proposed for them by Underdown (ca. 0.06-0.1/km2).

Second, the ‘Kuru Model’ is based on one case of a TSE decimating a single population. That is to say, not all cannibalism needs to result (or have resulted) in the development and rapid transmission of TSEs among its practitioners. This is a problem of extrapolating from one documented case among modern humans (ironically enough) to the entirety of Neanderthal groups. This is a common logical flaw whereby Neanderthals are not considered as comprising a multitude of groups of hunter-gatherers spread over a vast range and likely relatively well adapted to their local conditions. Neanderthals (or any fossil human species, for that matter) are not just a species of extinct hominins; that label refers to biologically similar hominins spread far and wide across Eurasia and whose primary adaptations to their environment were behavioral in nature. Think of it this way: do all Homo sapiens behave exactly the same across the world today? No, and neither did they in the past. This is why there is so much debate over how to satisfactorily define “behavioral modernity” and why we can’t just talk about modern humans acting in one way. The same was very likely true for Neanderthals.

Third, the two best-publicized cases of Neanderthal cannibalism (i.e., Krapina, and Moula-Guercy) go back to 100-80 kya at the most recent. There is, as far as I know, little in the way of strong evidence for widespread Neanderthal cannibalism after that; although clear anthropic modifications of Neanderthal remains have been documented at later sites, such as El Sidrón (Rosas et al. 2006), the case for cannibalism there has not been demonstrated since these have not been shown to be similar to those found on other animal remains at the site. Thus, there is little evidence that cannibalism was a widespread Neanderthal behavior around for the 50 ky that led up to their disappearance from the Eurasian fossil record. Interestingly, there is also some very suggestive evidence that Mousterian sites increased in density over the course of that time period (e.g., van Andel et al. 2003), as opposed to steadily decreasing in numbers as implied by the Underdown’s TSE model.

Fourth, there is clear evidence of perimortem processing of Homo sapiens remains with stone tools is also reported from the Aurignacian deposits of Brassempouy, Isturitz, Tarté and La Combe, where human teeth were forcefully removed from their gums and pierced to be transformed into ornaments (White et al. 2003), and in the Middle Stone Age deposits of Herto, where human skulls bear unambiguous traces of defleshing (Clark et al. 2003). Given that Underdown (in press: p. 3) states that TSEs can well have been transmitted “through cuts caused by stone tools used by infected and non-infected individuals,” shouldn’t we therefore also assume that TSEs would have been a major concern for modern humans as well?

Overall, then, while TSEs might well have been a problem for some groups of Neanderthals and some groups of Homo sapiens, there is no reason to assume that it was an especially important factor leading to the disappearance of Neanderthals as a distinct fossil hominin in the paleoanthropological record. I will say this, however: theoretically at least, Underdown’s scenario has the merit of having the potential to be tested empirically and independently through genetic studies. In that sense, it is a move forward in paleoanthropology.

on: http://averyremoteperiodindeed.blogspot.com/2008/03/mad-neanderthal-disease.html

Chiarelli B. 1
(1) Laboratori di Antropologia ed Etnologia, Università degli Studi di Firenze, Via del Proconsolo 12, 50122 Firenze, Italy

Received: 2 June 2001 Accepted: 3 June 2003
Abstract There are paleoecological evidences that Neanderthals ate the brains of deers and goats as well that of their own deceased. This would expose each individual and the population to the risks of contracting the Creutzfeldt Jacob disease. Those who consumed the remains of infected individual’s would then contract the disease and eventually infect others. If this is the case then the Neanderthal extinction could be attributed to spongiform encephalopathy and not to the cultural supremacy of the anatomically modern man.

Keywords neanderthals - cannibalism - spongiform encephalopathy

on: http://www.springerlink.com/content/k2932322j75728r7/

Cannibalism May Have Wiped Out Neanderthals
Jennifer Viegas, Discovery News




Feb. 27, 2008 -- A Neanderthal-eat-Neanderthal world may have spread a mad cow-like disease that weakened and reduced populations of the large Eurasian human, thereby contributing to its extinction, according to a new theory based on cannibalism that took place in more recent history.

Aside from illustrating that consumption of one's own species isn't exactly a healthy way to eat, the new theoretical model could resolve the longstanding mystery as to what caused Neanderthals, which emerged around 250,000 years ago, to disappear off the face of the Earth about 30,000 years ago.

"The story of Neanderthal extinction is one of the most intriguing in all of human evolution," author Simon Underdown told Discovery News. "Why did a large-brained, intelligent hominid that shared so many traits with us disappear?"

To resolve that question, Underdown, a lecturer in the Department of Anthropology at Oxford Brookes University, studied a well-documented tribal group, the Fore of Papua New Guinea, who practiced ritualistic cannibalism.

Gory evidence uncovered in a French cave in 1999 revealed Neanderthals likely practiced cannibalism. The 100,000-120,000 year-old bones discovered at the cave site of Moula-Guercy near the west bank of the Rhone river suggested a group of Neanderthals defleshed the bones of at least six other individuals and then broke the bones apart with a hammerstone and anvil to remove the marrow and brains.

Although it's not clear why Neanderthals may have eaten each other, research on the Fore determined that maternal kin of certain deceased Fore individuals used to dismember corpses and regarded some human flesh as a valuable food source.

Beginning in the early 1900's, anthropologists additionally began to take note of an affliction named Kuru among the Fore. By the 1960's, Kuru reached epidemic levels and killed over 1,100 people.

Subsequent investigations determined that Kuru was related to the Fore's cannibalistic activities and was a form of Transmissible Spongiform Encephalopathy, or TSE. This is a class of disease that includes mad cow disease. Underdown said TSE's have been in existence for possibly millions of years.

According to his new paper, published in the journal Medical Hypotheses, TSE's "cause brain tissue to take on an almost sponge-like appearance, caused by the formation of small holes during the development of the disease."

The disease's latter stages often result in severe mental impairment, loss of speech and an inability to move.

He created a model, based on the Kuru findings, to figure out how the spread of such a disease via cannibalism could reduce a population's size. For example, he calculated that within a hypothetical group of 15,000 individuals, such a disease could reduce the population to non-viable levels within 250 years.

on: http://dsc.discovery.com/news/2008/02/27/neanderthal-cannibalism.html

Hm, canibals, rapers of Cromagnon females, reduced frontal lobe.

About frontal lobe functions from wiki:

The frontal lobe contains most of the dopamine-sensitive neurons in the cerebral cortex. The dopamine system is associated with reward, attention, long-term memory, planning, and drive. Dopamine tends to limit and select sensory information arriving from the thalamus to the fore-brain. A report from the National Institute of Mental Health says a gene variant that reduces dopamine activity in the prefrontal cortex is related to poorer performance and inefficient functioning of that brain region during working memory tasks, and to slightly increased risk for schizophrenia.

or:

Tormented Souls, Diseased Brains

[ A case for the frontal brain | Images of violence | Conclusions | Next ]

Why sociopaths have these characteristics ? Are their brains different from those of normal people ? Do they display pathological alterations ?

Many studies have shown in the last 20 years that murderers and ultraviolent criminals have a startling evidence of brain disease. For example, in one such study, 20 of 31 confessed or sentenced murderers had specific neurological diagnoses. Some of the inmates had more than one disorders, and no subject was normal in all spheres. Among the diagnoses were schizophrenia, depression, epilepsy, alcoholism, alcoholic dementia, mental retardation, cerebral palsy, brain injury, dissociative disorders and others. More than 64 % of them appeared to have frontal lobe abnormalities. Fifty percent had brain atrophy and 40 % had EEG abnormalities. Almost 84 % of the subjects had been victims of severe physical and/or sexual abuse. The group of murderers included gang members, rapists, robbers, serial murderers, mass murderers, one subject who killed his infant son, and another who murdered three siblings.

In another study carried out in Canada in 1994, in the most violent group of 372 males imprisoned in a maximum-security mental hospital, 20 % had focal temporal abnormalities of the EEG, and 41 % had pathologic alterations of the brain structure in the temporal lobe. The corresponding rates for the least violent group were 2.4 % and 6.7 %, respectively, thus suggesting an important role of neurological damage in the genesis of violent personalities.

According to authors Nathaniel J. Pollone and James J. Hennessy, "[Various] studies over a period of nearly 40 years... suggest a relative incidence of neuropathology among violent offenders many times in excess of that found in the general population, at ratios ranging from a high of 31:1 in the case of homicide offenders through 21:1 among `habitual aggressive' offenders to a low of 4:1 in the case of `one-time aggressives.' We propose that, though such discrepancies do not confirm neuropathology as univariately causative of criminal aggression, neither is it reasonable to believe that they are simple artifacts of chance." (35th Annual Meeting of the Academy of Criminal Justice Sciences, Albuquerque, NM, March 14, 1998)

Although this has been always a very controversial subject, many researchers think that there is now a compelling case for a substrate of brain disease in violent criminals; and this has very important consequences for many things, from the point of view of the law to the prospect of effective prevention and treatment of sociopathy.
A Case for the Frontal Brain

Since sociopathic individuals have marked alterations in their relation to other human beings, it is only natural that we should first seek whether the part of their brains responsible for this has some significant abnormality.

Much of the behavior which makes possible stable and adequate social relations is controlled by the part of the brain called frontal lobe, which is located in the most anterior part of the brain hemispheres. All social primates have highly developed frontal brains, and human beings have the largest one of all. Self-control, planning, judgment, the balance of individual versus social needs, and many other essential functions underlying effective social intercourse are mediated by the frontal structures of the brain (see Dr. Silvia Cardoso's enlightening article on "The External Architecture of the Brain" in Brain & Mind Magazine's first issue, to understand what's the frontal brain)

Why the frontal brain seems to be so important in the genesis of antisocial individuals ?

Research with animals has shown that the right orbitofrontal cortex is involved in fear conditioning. For instance, when a rat is punished with an electrical shock every time a light blinks in its cage, it develops a fear association between the stimulus and the punishment. Normal humans learn very early in life to avoid antisocial behavior because they are punished for it and because they have the brain circuits to associate fear of punishment (feeling emotion) to behavior suppression. This seems to be a key element in the development of personality. When there is no punishment, or when the person is unable to be conditioned by fear, due to a lesion in the orbitofrontal cortex, for example, or due to lowered neural activity in this area, then it develops an antisocial personality.

We have now a direct way of visualizing brain function, which has lead to a remarkable explosion in our knowledge about the inner workings of the psychopath's brain in the last two to three years:
Images of Violence
Functional images of the brain, such as those provided by PET (positron emission tomography) have been used to corroborate the existence of neurological deficits in the frontal lobe in sociopaths. PET shows computer reconstructed transversal sections of the brain, showing in vivid color the level of metabolic activity of neurons. This is achieved by injecting radioactivity-marked glucose molecules into the patient's blood flow and seeing how much of it is incorporated into living brain cells. The more active the cells are (when they are processing information, for example), more intense will be the image at that point (see my article "PET: A New Window Into the Brain", in the first issue of Brain & Mind Magazine, to understand better how this technique works).

Positron Emission Tomography (PET) scanner obtains sectional images of the living brain, using color to depict degree of activity. Crump Institute for Biological Imaging, University of California at Los Angeles.

Using the PET technique, American medical researchers Adrian Raine and colleagues have been studying murderers, with startling results. They found that 41 murderers have a much decreased level of brain functioning in the prefrontal cortex than normal persons, indicating a deficit related to violence. In other words, even when no visible pathological alteration was present, frontal damage was apparent by a abnormal lower activity of the brain in that area. "Damage to this brain region," Raine noted, "can result in impulsivity, loss of self-control, immaturity, altered emotionality, and the inability to modify behavior, which can all in turn facilitate aggressive acts." Other abnormalities observed by the PET study of the murderers' brain included reduced neural metabolism in the superior parietal gyrus, left angular gyrus, and the corpus callosum, and abnormal asymmetries of activity in the amygdala, thalamus, and medial temporal lobe. It is probable that these effects are related to violence and criminality; because some of these structures are part of the so-called limbic brain, which processes emotions and emotional behavior (please see "Limbic System: The Center of Emotions" in Brain & Mind Magazine)

One interesting aspect of Dr. Raine's research is that he correlated the PET brain images to the murderer''s personal history, in order to ascertain whether they were subjected to trauma, physical or sexual abuse, neglect, poverty, when they were children (a deprived environment for the development of personality). Of the murderers, 12 had suffered significant abuse or deprivation. It was discovered that murderer's coming from non-deprived households had much larger deficits in the orbitofrontal area of the brain (14 % on the average) than normal people and murderer's coming from deprived environments.

The initial controlled studies carried out by Raine and colleagues have been confirmed by a series of PET-based investigations with sociopathic individuals and violent criminals. In a study in 1994, 17 patients with diagnoses of personality disorder were subjected to PET scams. The researchers proved that there was a strong inverse correlation between a life history of aggressive impulse difficulties and regional metabolism in the frontal cortex. Six of these patients were antisocial, the rest had several personality disorders (borderline, dependent and narcissistic). PET was used again in 1995 to evaluate brain glucose metabolism in eight normal subjects and eight psychiatric patients with a history of repetitive violent behavior. The authors observed that "seven of the patients showed widespread areas of low brain metabolism, particularly in medial temporal and prefrontal cortices than did normal comparison subjects. These regions have been implicated as substrates for aggression and impulsivity, and their dysfunction may have contributed to the patients' violent behavior". More recently (1997), PET brain imaging technology found that psychopaths differed from nonpsychopaths in the pattern of relative cerebral blood flow during processing of emotional words. Acquired personality changes due to brain injury are also accompanied by a decrease in the neural activity in the frontal area

Indirect evidence of the role of prefrontal cortex in psychopathic behavior is coming from other experiments as well. In Canada, a team headed by Dominique LaPierre compared 30 psychopaths to 30 non-psychopathic criminals, using tests that evaluate the function of two parts of the prefrontal cortex: the orbitofrontal and the frontal ventromedial areas. The results showed that "the psychopaths were significantly impaired on all the orbitofrontal- ventromedial tasks", but not in the function of other areas of the frontal cortex. The similarities between psychopaths and patients with prefrontal cortex damage surfaced in several areas of the study. "Both the psychopath and the orbitofrontal or ventromedial frontal patient show an exaggerated preoccupation with sexual matters, acting in a promiscuous and impersonal maladaptive way," observed the researchers. "Both are remarkable for their lack of social and ethical judgment. Both neglect long-term consequences of their actions, choosing immediate gratification over careful planning."

So, there is a reasonable body of coherent evidence that sociopaths have a dysfunction of the frontal brain. Why and when this dysfunction appears is totally unknown, thus far.

on: http://www.cerebromente.org.br/n07/doencas/disease_i.htm

Neanderthals, archaics, and other peoples of the Middle Paleolithic were not very smart and used
simple stones for tools. In fact, they constructed and made essentially the same stone tools over and
over again for perhaps 200,000 years, until around 35,000 B.P., with little variation or consideration of
alternatives (Binford, 1982; Gowlett, 1984; Mellars, 1989). Neanderthals greatly lacked in creativity,
initiative, imagination, and tended to create simple stone tools that served a single purpose.
As neatly summed up by an ardent defender of Neanderthal cognitive capabilities (Hayden,
1993, p. 139), “as a rule, there is no evidence of private ownership or food storage, no evidence for
the use of economic resources for status or political competition, no elaborate burials, no ornaments
or other status display items, no skin garments requiring intensive labor to produce, no tools requiring
high energy investments, no intensive regional exchange for rare items like sea shells or amber,
no competition for labor to produce economic surpluses and no corporate art or labor intensive rituals
in deep cave recesses to impress onlookers and help attract labor.”
Neanderthals tended to live in the “here and now,” with little ability to think about or consider
the distant future
(Binford, 1973, 1982; Dennell, 1985; Mellars, 1989, 1996); the only notable exception,
the future life after death.
This notable dichotomy is in part a function of the differential evolution of the frontal versus
the temporal lobes. The frontal lobes are the senior executive of the brain and are responsible for
initiative, goal formation, long term planning, the generation of multiple alternatives, and the consideration
of multiple alternative consequences (Joseph, 1986a, 1988a, 1999b). The frontal lobes are the
source of creativity and imagination
, whereas the temporal lobes are the seat of the soul. It is the
temporal lobes which were maximally developed in archaic and Neanderthals, whereas the frontal
lobe would increase in size by a third in the transition from archaic humans to Cro-Magnon woman
and man.

from: http://brainmind.com/SpiritualEvolution.pdf

So much about Neanderthal for now.
 
Laura said:
Well.... that was intense. I'm going to re-read Bradley's book. Maybe we ought to run that article on sott?

I just read his whole page, and would think twice about it. As you go down, Bradley goes off into some pretty iffy rants about defending western society against the semitic invaders, including reviving the "true meaning of fascism". (???) He has already created a flag using an ancient variation on the swastika and is calling for a younger more energetic person to lead the movement. You can see his intent, but without a knowledge of ponerology, it would just go the way of every other movement. He also ends with a want-ad for a new companion as his marriage has broken up. It's kinda creepy.

It's like the guy (David somebody?)who did all the research on the Dresden bombings. That book was written very well, bringing many unknown facts to light. And then he went right off the deep end. That same feeling is here.

Anyone else's take on his page would be welcome.

Herondancer
 
herondancer said:
Laura said:
Well.... that was intense. I'm going to re-read Bradley's book. Maybe we ought to run that article on sott?

I just read his whole page, and would think twice about it. As you go down, Bradley goes off into some pretty iffy rants about defending western society against the semitic invaders, including reviving the "true meaning of fascism". (???) He has already created a flag using an ancient variation on the swastika and is calling for a younger more energetic person to lead the movement. You can see his intent, but without a knowledge of ponerology, it would just go the way of every other movement. He also ends with a want-ad for a new companion as his marriage has broken up. It's kinda creepy.

It's like the guy (David somebody?)who did all the research on the Dresden bombings. That book was written very well, bringing many unknown facts to light. And then he went right off the deep end. That same feeling is here.

Anyone else's take on his page would be welcome.

Herondancer

My impression as well. And below that, interviews with Michael Collins Piper. In short, he may have some good research, but as for the rest, iffy at best.
 
Okay, scratch that idea. Good thing many eyes are looking!
 
The problem with Bradley is that he seems to be obsessed with warfare and sex that distorts an otherwise interesting hypothesis about neanderthals and who their subsequent descendants really are. So while I personally think he may have been on to something regarding neanderthals, I also think he is crazy or deliberately presenting info in manner to mislead. He did at some point write that he believes his information was "given" to him during several encounters with a white light. UFO type encounters were implied but he "leaves it to his readers to decide". He has to be taken with a grain of salt as even in his writings he seems somewhat odd or unstable.


brainwave
 
I just got ahold of Bradley's book and started reading -- I'm only on the first chapter, but so far there seems to be some good information mixed in with a lot of junk. I'm interested in his actual data, but he seems to make lots of unwarranted assumptions about various things, so I'm going to have to read it slowly.

On another note, I noticed the following paragraph today in an article entitled 'Neanderthal Genome Decoded' by Zach Zorich (Archaeology July/August 2010, p. 37), regarding the recent comparison of the Neanderthal DNA with that of five Homo Sapiens:

The research team cautions against drawing any racist conclusions based on their results. “It’s not as if there are places in the genome where all non-Africans have Neanderthal ancestry and all Africans do not have Neanderthal ancestry,” says geneticist David Reich of Harvard University. “In fact, each non-African today seems to have their Neanderthal ancestry in a different place in the genome.” He also points out that there is no single Neanderthal gene that all non-Africans share.

This seems to provide more reason to question the hypothesis that Neanderthal DNA in human genomes is the result of a single admixture event involving a homogeneous group of non-African humans during an out-of-Africa migration. But again, five people is a really small sample on which to be basing any hypotheses in the first place.
 
Just wanted to mention that I finished Bradley's book Chosen People From The Caucasus last night. It's a mixed bag IMO, by someone who asks a lot of the right questions, even if he doesn't always get the right answers. There are parts that I think that are very problematic, but also parts that are informative.

Bradley's basic hypothesis is that Caucasoids are the result of admixture between modern humans and Neanderthals, the primary locus of the latter being places in the Caucasus region, and that they have inherited psychosexual maladaptations from their Neanderthal forebears which developed under harsh ice age conditions. These maladaptations, variously manifested, are in turn responsible for the ways in which Caucasoids have oppressed the other 'races' throughout history.

Regarding the Jews, Bradley correctly distinguishes between Sephardim and Ashkenazim. His hypotheses about the first of these two groups is problematic. He asserts that the original Jews were Indo-European (Neanderthal hybrids), and that their Biblical history shows as much, such as when he equates the Nephilim of the Bible with Neanderthals who raped human women and produced giant hybrid offspring, the scapegoats of the Bible ('hairy ones') with Neanderthal hybrids, and explains the marriage of Abraham to his half-sister Sara as a way to maintain the Neanderthal line (Hagar, Abraham's other wife, having been 'pure' modern human). He argues that this group migrated south to the Middle East, adopted a Semitic language, and ultimately founded the historical Israel which lasted until the Roman sacking of 70 AD, at which point the Jewish diaspora occurred, largely into Spain and southern France (this final part is historically accurate). Bradley doesn't offer a lot of compelling evidence for this part of his story, and it is largely conjecture.

Bradley's coverage of the Ashkenazim is much better, and is largely accurate as far as I can tell. He gives an overview of the Khazar conversion to Judaism, followed by the break-up of the Khazar empire and subsequent invasion of the Mongols which pushed the Khazar (Ashkenazi) Jews west into Eastern Europe, where they made up a large portion of the Hungarian population and established enclaves in other Eastern European regions, where they were eventually discovered by Sephardic Jews, much to their surprise, in numbers that ultimately dwarfed their own population. Bradley mentions that Russian anti-Semitism may have been a reaction to an earlier period of Khazar slave-raiding (the word 'slave' ultimately deriving from the ethnic term 'Slav'). He argues that the Khazars, being Neanderthal hybrids themselves (how he reaches that conclusion is a bit murky), may have responded well to a religion which had its roots in Neanderthal psychology.
 
Neandertals!

Several interesting hypothesis regarding neandertals.

_http://themandus.org/

hypothesis is that neandertals were the ultimate top predator and practically devoured our race into extinction in the Levant some 50,000 years ago.

_http://www.rdos.net/eng/asperger.htm

Claims that neandertals were the main force behind domestication and interbred with us. That they also left us the legacy of sexual diversity and various "disorders" like asperger's.

I think Michael Bradley has already been commented on elsewhere here.
 
News item on the topic:

http://onlineathens.com/stories/090810/new_705579694.shtml
UGA Researchers Compare Neanderthals to Humans

Lee Shearer
Online Athens
Wed, 08 Sep 2010

Scientists have long tried to tease out the secrets of what makes us human by comparing human behavior to how other species behave, such as chimpanzees.

But now a research team hopes to get at the question another way - by studying the DNA of Neanderthals, early hominids that were similar to humans, said Richard "Ed" Green, a computational biologist at the University of California at Santa Cruz.

"What is clear is that our closest extinct relative is the Neanderthal," Green told an overflow crowd of University of Georgia professors and students in a Coverdell Hall lecture room Tuesday. "They are way more similar to humans than anything else."

Green is part of the research team, led by Svante Paabo of Germany's Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, that partially sequenced the genome of Neanderthals using DNA from bones buried in a Croatian cave nearly 40,000 years ago.

The team already found one big surprise when they compared the Neanderthal DNA to modern humans.

"We know they were reproductively compatible because you are 1 to 4 percent Neanderthal (genetically)," Green said.

In other words, modern humans and Neanderthals interbred, said Green, a Conyers native who got his undergraduate degree at UGA.

Neanderthals arose about 200,000 years ago but went extinct about 30,000 years ago - and no one really knows why. Modern humans date to 130,000 years ago - so humans and Neanderthals coexisted for tens of thousands of years.

But the differences in Neanderthal and human genomes may tell scientists more than the similarities as they try to understand how humans got to be the way we are, Green said.

Some of the genetic differences in humans and Neanderthals seem to be in regions of the human genome involved in autism - a little-understood condition that affects people's ability to interact with other people.

Green and other scientists speculate that modern humans may have genetic mutations absent in Neanderthals that somehow gave us the ability to build bigger social groups than our extinct relatives, Green said.

I'm beginning to wonder about all this. What if Neanderthals and modern humans were NOT "reproductively compatible" and the presence of similar genes simply represents the last common ancestor?
 
...anyway every day we are closer and closer to dates predicted by Cs :)

"But Dr Michael Petraglia, of Oxford University, and colleagues say stone artefacts found in the Arabian Peninsula and India point to an exodus starting about 70,000 to 80,000 years ago - and perhaps even earlier."

http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/science-environment-11327442
 
New piece of data:

http://www.sott.net/articles/show/217600-The-brains-of-Neanderthals-and-modern-humans-developed-differently

Researcher at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology in Leipzig, Germany have documented species differences in the pattern of brain development after birth that are likely to contribute to cognitive differences between modern humans and Neanderthals.

Whether cognitive differences exist between modern humans and Neanderthals is the subject of contentious disputes in anthropology and archaeology. Because the brain size range of modern humans and Neanderthals overlap, many researchers previously assumed that the cognitive capabilities of these two species were similar. Among humans, however, the internal organization of the brain is more important for cognitive abilities than its absolute size is. The brain's internal organization depends on the tempo and mode of brain development.

Based on detailed measurements of internal shape changes of the braincase during individual growth, a team of scientists from the MPI has shown that these are differences in the patterns of brain development between humans and Neanderthals during a critical phase for cognitive development.

Discussions about the cognitive abilities of fossil humans usually focus on material culture (e.g. the complexity of the stone tool production process) and endocranial volumes. "The interpretation of the archaeological evidence remains controversial, and the brain-size ranges of Neanderthals and modern humans overlap," says Jean-Jacques Hublin, director of the Department of Human Evolution at the MPI-EVA in Leipzig where the research was conducted. Hublin adds, "our findings show how biological differences between modern humans and Neanderthals may be linked to behavioural differences inferred from the archaeological record."

Nature of the evidence: As the brain does not fossilize, for fossil skulls, only the imprints of the brain and its surrounding structures in the bone (so called "endocasts") can be studied. The researchers used state-of-the-art statistical methods to compare shape changes of virtual endocasts extracted from computed-tomographic scans. The distinct globular shape of the braincase of adult Homo sapiens is largely the result of a brain development phase that is not present in Neanderthals.

One of the key pieces of evidence was the skull reconstruction of a Neanderthal newborn. In 1914, a team of French archaeologists had excavated the skeleton of a Neanderthal baby at the rock shelter of Le Moustier in the Dordogne. The original bones of the skeleton had been lost to science for more than 90 years, until they were rediscovered among museum collections by Bruno Maureille and the museum staff. The restored original baby bones are now on permanent display at the Musée National de Préhistoire in Les Eyzies-de-Tayac-Sireuil. The museum's director Jean-Jacques Cleyet-Merle made it possible to scan the delicate fragments using a high-resolution computed-tomographic scanner (µCT). Using computers at the Max Planck Institute's virtual reality lab in Leipzig, Philipp Gunz and Simon Neubauer then reconstructed the Neanderthal baby from the digital pieces, like in a three-dimensional jigsaw puzzle. "When we compare the skulls of a Neanderthal and a modern human newborn, the Neanderthal's face is already larger at the time of birth. However, most shape differences of the internal braincase develop after birth," explains Gunz. Both Neanderthals and modern human neonates have elongated braincases at the time of birth, but only modern human endocasts change to a more globular shape in the first year of life. Modern humans and Neanderthals therefore reach large adult brain sizes via different developmental pathways.

In a related study the same team of MPI researchers had previously shown that the developmental patterns of the brain were remarkably similar between chimpanzees and humans after the first year of life, but differed markedly directly after birth. "We interpret those aspects of development that are shared between modern humans, Neanderthals, and chimpanzees as conserved," explains Simon Neubauer. "This developmental pattern has probably not changed since the last common ancestor of chimpanzees and humans several million years ago." In the first year of life, modern humans, but not Neanderthals, depart from this ancestral pattern of brain development.

Establishing when the species differences between Neanderthal and modern human adults emerge during development was critical for understanding whether differences in the pattern of brain development might underlie potential cognitive differences. As the differences between modern humans and Neanderthals are most prominent in the period directly after birth, they likely have implications for the neuronal and synaptic organization of the developing brain.

The development of cognitive abilities during individual growth is linked to the maturation of the underlying wiring pattern of the brain; around the time of birth, the neural circuitry is sparse in humans, and clinical studies have linked even subtle alterations in early brain development to changes in the neural wiring patterns that affect behaviour and cognition. The connections between diverse brain regions that are established during this period in modern humans are important for higher-order social, emotional, and communication functions. It is therefore unlikely that Neanderthals saw the world as we do.

The new study shows that modern humans have a unique pattern of brain development after birth, which separates us from our closest relatives, the Neanderthals. This uniquely modern human pattern of early brain development is particularly interesting in light of the recent breakthroughs in the Neanderthal genome project. A comparison of Neanderthal and modern human genomes revealed several regions with strong evidence for positive selection within Homo sapiens, i.e. the selection occurred after the split between modern humans and Neanderthals. Three among these are likely to be critical for brain development, as they affect mental and cognitive development.

"Our findings have two important implications," says Philipp Gunz. "We have discovered differences in the patterns of brain development that might contribute to cognitive differences between modern humans and Neanderthals. Maybe more importantly, however, this discovery will tell us more about our own species than about Neanderthals; we hope that our findings will help to identify the function of some genes that show evidence for recent selection in modern humans."

Web_Zoom.jpg

The brains of Neanderthals and modern humans are very similar at the time of birth. A reconstruction of a Neanderthal baby is compared to a modern human newborn. While the face of the Neanderthal is already larger than in a modern human at the time of birth, their brain shapes and volumes are very similar. Internal casts of brain cavities of skulls (Neanderthal: red; modern humans: blue) provide information about the relative size and form of the brain.
 
Herr Eisenheim said:
are there any viable data on prevalence of the psychopathy amongst African populations?

from the NIH:

Half of the population of the region is made up of children below age of 15 years. It is estimated that, of those aged 0-9 years, about 3% suffer from a mental disorder. Many children suffer from poor psychosocial development because of neglect by their mothers and other caretakers. Brain damage is one of the main causes of serious mental retardation.

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1489826/

so it looks like the rates in Africa will closely match the rates in Europeans
 
Just an interesting side-note: Being American, having lived and worked in ethnically diverse areas, including more affluent suburbs, I have noticed time and time again that Jewish men in particular do seem to have what I termed a "Nordic fetish", meaning that when they marry or have children outside of their own community, it is almost exclusively with "nordic" type blonde haired and blue eyed women. Now I do realize that a lot of human cultures, especially non-white and/ or non Western European ones, have a particularity for the Nordic phenotype, and my theory supports the research here, that this is purposefully ingrained in all of us, humans, since ancient times, to see the Nordic-Aryan type as some kind of "master race" (probably ingrained by Reptilians and/or the extraterrestrial 'Nordic' Orion races originally and propagated throughout history by various groups, including the descendants of STS Kantekkians in the past tens of thousands of years).
Of course as of recently, the Jewish ethnic group is able to marry into the upper echelons of white society with no problems and my theory is that it may be a subconscious drive on their part to "upgrade" their genetics, and based on the C's comments, possibly try to return to a more "pure" Aryan geno/phenotype in future generations, much as the biblical Hebrews were. Just a few comments and suggestions.



Laura said:
A couple more possible clues:

http://globalfire.tv/nj/03en/jews/schizo.htm
Physician Claims Jews are Schizo Carriers
Subtitle: "Is Mental Illness the Jewish Disease?"

It is NOT posted on the Internet. It's from Psychiatric News, published by the American Psychiatric Association. Date of publication: Oct. 25, 1972. Go to your local library.

Evidence that Jews are carriers of schizophrenia is disclosed in a paper prepared for the American Journal of Psychiatry by Dr. Arnold A. Hutschnecker, the New York psychiatrist who once treated President Nixon.

In a study entitled "Mental Illness: The Jewish Disease" Dr. Hutschnecker said that although all Jews are not mentally ill, mental illness is highly contagious and Jews are the principal sources of infection.

Dr. Hutschnecker stated that every Jew is born with the seeds of schizophrenia and it is this fact that accounts for the world-wide persecution of Jews.

"The world would be more compassionate toward the Jews if it was generally realized that Jews are not responsible for their condition." Dr. Hutschnecker said. "Schizophrenia is the fact that creates in Jews a compulsive desire for persecution."

Dr. Hutschnecker pointed out that mental illness peculiar to Jews is manifested by their inability to differentiate between right and wrong. He said that, although Jewish canonical law recognizes the virtues of patience, humility and integrity, Jews are aggressive, vindictive and dishonest.

"While Jews attack non-Jewish Americans for racism, Israel is the most racist country in the world," Dr. Hutschnecker said.

Jews, according to Dr. Hutschnecker, display their mental illness through their paranoia. He explained that the paranoiac not only imagines that he is being persecuted but deliberately creates situations which will make persecution a reality.

Dr. Hutschnecker said that all a person need do to see Jewish paranoia in action is to ride on the New York subway. Nine times out of ten, he said, the one who pushes you out of the way will be a Jew.

"The Jew hopes you will retaliate in kind and when you do he can tell himself you are anti-Semitic."

During World War II, Dr. Hutschnecker said, Jewish leaders in England and the United States knew about the terrible massacre of the Jews by the Nazis. But, he stated, when State Department officials wanted to speak out against the massacre, they were silenced by organized Jewry. Organized Jewry, he said, wanted the massacre to continue in order to arouse the world's sympathy.

Dr. Hutschnecker likened the Jewish need to be persecuted to the kind of insanity where the afflicted person mutilates himself. He said that those who mutilate themselves do so because they want sympathy for themselves. But, he added, such persons reveal their insanity by disfiguring themselves in such a way as to arouse revulsion rather than sympathy.

Dr. Hutschnecker noted that the incidence of mental illness has increased in the United States in direct proportion to the increase in the Jewish population.

"The great Jewish migration to the United States began at the end of the nineteenth century," Dr. Hutschnecker said. "In 1900 there were 1,058,135 Jews in the United States; in 1970 there were 5,868,555; an increase of 454.8%. In 1900 there were 62,112 persons confined in public mental hospitals in the United States; in 1970 there were 339,027, in increase of 445.7%. In the same period the U.S. population rose from 76,212,368 to 203,211,926, an increase of 166.6%. Prior to the influx of Jews from Europe the United States was a mentally healthy nation. But this is no longer true."

Dr. Hutschnecker substantiated his claim that the United States was no longer a mentally healthy nation by quoting Dr. David Rosenthal, chief of the laboratory of psychology at the National Institute of Mental Health, who recently estimated that more than 60,000,000 people in the United States suffer from some form of "schizophrenic spectrum disorder." Noting that Dr. Rosenthal is Jewish, Dr. Hutschnecker said that Jews seem to take a perverse pride in the spread of mental illness.

Dr. Hutschnecker said that the word "schizophrenia" was given to mental disease by dr. Eugen Blueler, a Swiss psychiatrist, in 1911. Prior to that time it had been known as "dementia praecox," the name used by its discoverer, Dr. Emil Kraepelin. Later, according to Dr. Hutschnecker, the same disease was given the name "neurosis" by Dr. Sigmund Freud.

"The symptoms of schizophrenia were recognized almost simultaneously by Bleuler, Kraepelin and Freud at a time when Jews were moving into the affluent middle class," Dr. Hutschnecker said. "Previously they had been ignored as a social and racial entity by the physicians of that era. They became clinically important when they began to intermingle with non-Jews."

Dr. Hutschnecker said that research by Dr. Jacques S. Gottlieb of Wayne State University indicates that schizophrenia is caused by deformity in the alpha-two-globulin protein, which in schizophrenics is corkscrew-shaped. The deformed protein is apparently caused by a virus which, Dr. Hutschnecker believes, Jews transmit to non-Jews with whom they come in contact. He said that because those descended from Western European peoples have not built up an immunity to the virus they are particularly vulnerable to the disease.

"There is no doubt in my mind," Dr. Hutschnecker said, "that Jews have infected the American people with schizophrenia. Jews are carriers of the disease and it will reach epidemic proportions unless science develops a vaccine to counteract it."

A Jew learns as part of his sacred Bible and Talmud studies that crimes against Gentiles, such as genocide, mass murder, child abuse etc. are considered holy services to God. The Bible and the Talmud, the holy books of hate, mandating innocent Jewish youngsters to hate, to murder, to deceit, to expel, to rob non-Jews in the name of God. This leaves not only a dent on their mental structure, but turns their soul into a festering spiritual deformity.

They live and work together with non-Jews in communities throughout the world, liking their Gentile neighbours, enjoying their culture and even at times falling in love with members of the Goyim opposite sex. However, because of the strict law and restrictions (based on racial purity) imposed upon them, they are forced to grow up and develop in a world in which they learn the skills of obeying one command and living a double life. The dilemma the ordinary Jew faces, is that he has to obey the Rabbis' interpretation of God's laws. They educated the young minds of the Jewish nation to despise, to subjugate, to look down upon and even to hate the non-Jewish world. Failing to follow these holy commands results, as they are taught, in severe punishment by God. This must result naturally in mental aberration amongst the Jewish community.

Loathsome example-patients of this serious mental illness, that befalls Jews, are America's neo-conservatives who seem to take joy in waging wars in the Middle East.

And:

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7973467
Schizophr Bull. 1994;20(3):507-17.
Medical conditions in Ashkenazi schizophrenic pedigrees.

Goodman AB.

Epidemiology and Health Services Research Laboratory, Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg, NY 10962.
Abstract

To limit the genetic heterogeneity of schizophrenia, this study focused on the widely extended pedigrees of Ashkenazi Jewish schizophrenia probands. The hypothesis posed is that the increased prevalence among the Ashkenazim of the rare lysosomal enzyme disorders, Tay Sachs disease (TDS), caused by low levels of hexosaminidase A, and Gaucher's disease (GD), caused by low levels of glucocerebrosidase, might contribute to the demonstrated increased vulnerability to schizophrenia in this ethnic group. Signs and symptoms characterizing the candidate illnesses were systematically queried by the family history method. Rates and relative risks for symptoms characterizing these disorders and for several nonautosomal illnesses associated with TSD and/or GD (i.e., amyotrophic lateral sclerosis and Hodgkin's disease, leukemia and lymphoma) are significantly elevated in the schizophrenia pedigrees, compared to controls. The conditions with elevated rates and risks have been associated with chromosomal regions 1q21 and 15q23-q24. These areas are suggested as candidate regions for future targeted deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA) research in schizophrenia.

PMID: 7973467 [PubMed - indexed for MEDLINE]
 
Back
Top Bottom