Trauma therapies: EMDR, Imaginal Rescripting and Imaginal Exposure

Oxajil

Ambassador
Ambassador
FOTCM Member
Most people have probably been through some kind of traumatic experiences. A person can develop PTSD from a single traumatic event or multiple traumatic events. This diagnosis is given when a person meets certain criteria as outlined by the DSM-5 (or the ICD). The criteria include symptoms such as nightmares, flashbacks, avoidant behavior or intrusive thoughts related to the traumatic event. However, not everyone meets all the criteria for PTSD. Unprocessed trauma can still have a significant effect on an individual's well-being without it leading to typical diagnostic symptoms. Current evidence-based trauma therapies or techniques include EMDR, Imaginal Rescripting and Imaginal Exposure. I thought I'd open this thread so we can share our thoughts about EMDR and the imaginal trauma techniques, and imaginal research in general.

Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing (EMDR)
In short, EMDR is a form of therapy that involves recalling and holding in mind a traumatic memory's most distressing moment while simultaneously engaging in tasks that challenge the working memory, such as tracking the therapist's finger movements, listening to alternating sounds, counting, or following on-screen visuals. Research indicates that tasks incorporating eye movements are particularly effective in this process (I'll have to look for the paper and will post it here once I've found it). EMDR is believed to initiate trauma (re)processing during which people often experience a range of emotions, bodily sensations and/or gain new insights or perspectives on their traumatic experience.

How it exactly works, however, is still unclear. This recent paper outlines the current hypotheses:

Adaptive Information Processing model

Since EMDR's introduction, several mechanistic hypotheses have been proposed to explain the effects of bilateral stimulation in EMDR. One of them is the adaptive information processing (AIP) model. This suggests that EMDR involves a re-setting of the system that processes and stores events during stressful situations, which reduces distress and negative emotions triggered by traumatic experiences.

Working Memory Theory

Another theory to explain the effects of bilateral stimulation is the working memory theory, which proposes that by taxing the working memory, eye movements permanently reduce the vividness and emotionality of aversive memories. This theory has been supported by laboratory studies in healthy individuals.

Orientation Response Model

Finally, the orientating response model suggests that bilateral stimulation triggers an investigatory reflex, reducing negative emotions and enhancing awareness, facilitating exploratory behavior, and potentially improving cognitive processes. However, a recent meta-analysis that included dismantling studies comparing EMDR with and without eye movements, found no benefit of eye movements, casting doubt about the superiority of EMDR to trauma-focused treatments without eye movements, such as exposure therapy or cognitive behavioral therapy with a trauma-focus.

As a psychologist and after being trained, I've started using EMDR according to the protocol, and it's quite amazing how effective it can be. I'll share some examples below (I've changed some personal details):

I did an EMDR session with a client whose mother was sadistic. One of the memories we worked on is when her mother told her and her sister that they have to start clean the house. While she was vacuuming, her mother grabbed the cord of the vacuum cleaner and tried to strangle her with it. At first she thought she was playing around, but the cord became tighter and she tried to loosen it with her hands. Soon after, she blacked out and fell on the table. She was told by her mother to tell others that it was an accident. The most distressing moment or the image she chose for the EMDR session was seeing the cord around her neck.

During the session, she noticed she gained more distance from the image. Before, she was 'in' it, and close to the end of the session it was as if she was looking at the memory as a 'bystander'. At the end, all she saw was just a vacuum cleaner on the floor. The memory lost its vividness and emotional intensity and in a way it became a regular memory. She still knows what happened, but she gained a different perspective ("She's in no position to have power over me. I'm stronger than her. I'm not afraid of her."). While many tears flowed during the sessions, she became more emotionally stable. The memory doesn't haunt her anymore, and she became much better at setting boundaries with her mother.

Another example:

A client had only a handful of memories of his father. We worked on the following memory. When he was 4 years old his dad hit him in the face while they were in the garden. His dad then walked back into the house. He didn't understand why he hit him. Later, his dad came back and apologized to him. He was actually quite positive about his father during our talks, saying that at least he apologized. This was also an event that wasn't very high on the list. But interestingly, compared to the other traumatic events we covered, it was this one that was the most emotional and had a lot of layers to it. The most distressing moment or the image he chose for the EMDR session was the angry face of his father as he was about to hit him.

This is how the image changed during EMDR: "I'm sitting with my head down while my face is hurting and my nose is bleeding profusely" to "I'm standing now" to "I'm standing with my hands in fists and I'm looking in the direction my father walked away" to "My father's back and apologizing, but I'm just accepting it for the sake of it, because it doesn't excuse him hitting me. That was not okay. I also have this strong sense that it wasn't even my fault. And, on top of that, I was just a kid." This revealed a different emotion that he didn't express during our talks but only after EMDR: anger towards his father. The next layer we uncovered was that of grief. The next step is to process these emotions, possibly incorporating Imaginal Rescripting.

EMDR can also be used for preverbal trauma. See this article for example (translated):

Impactful Events
Recently, lead therapist Nancy encountered a case involving preverbal trauma during a work supervision session with a behavioral scientist. She recounts: "This colleague presented a case of a client, a young teenager, who exhibited behavioral problems and suffered from anxieties. The client was referred through school, but neither the school nor the parents could understand the origin of this behavior. Even the child couldn't explain it well. My colleague then began diagnostic research, which always includes looking at possible impactful events the child has experienced. Conversations with the parents revealed that this child had undergone many surgeries as a baby. After each operation, the parents had to ensure daily that the wounds remained clean. This was very painful for the baby but necessary for proper wound healing.

"Unsafe World"
The diagnostic research is still ongoing," Nancy explains, "but a likely explanation for the symptoms is that this child, due to the medical procedures and painful wound care, came to believe that the world is unsafe, that others are not to be trusted, and even your own parents can cause you pain. When you have to undergo many painful medical procedures as a young child and don't understand why it's necessary because you don't have words for it, it's a very frightening situation. Later, your nervous system can more quickly give you the feeling that danger is imminent during stress, even if that's not the case in the current situation."

Physical Memory
A preverbal trauma occurs in the preverbal phase of a child, before the child could speak. The fact that a child can't put it into words and doesn't seem to have an active memory of it doesn't necessarily mean there's no trauma, EMDR therapist Maartje knows. "Even if a child can't speak yet: the body also stores impactful experiences. I see this clearly in the treatment of preverbal trauma: a child says they don't remember anything, but when evoking the impactful event, you clearly see physical changes. The body becomes tense and cramped, or the body starts trembling or shaking."

Activating Traumatic Memory
If during the diagnostic phase there's a suspicion of a connection between the symptoms and experienced impactful events, treating the underlying trauma is beneficial. For treatment, it's important to activate this traumatic memory. Nancy: "We ask the parents to write a story about the impactful event, as detailed and sensory as possible. So: the operating room was ice cold, bright lights were shining, the doctors wore blue coats, you got a mask with a nasty smell. When the story is finished, the mother or father reads it to the child in the presence of the EMDR therapist. We closely monitor the physical reaction. Often the child sits on their mother's or father's lap. Interestingly, they often also feel what's happening in their child's body, for example, a very rapid heartbeat that then calms down."

Processing through EMDR
Evoking the unpleasant memories is accompanied by EMDR therapy, therapist Maartje explains. "We can effectively treat a preverbal trauma with EMDR therapy, even if the child is now much older and can speak well. EMDR stands for Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing. During treatment, we bring the memories to the surface as vividly as possible. At the same time, we provide visual stimulation with hand movements, sounds, or light. The child closely follows these movements with their eyes. I ask the child: what comes up or what do you notice? Often the child says they don't feel much negative, but I still see expressions of physical memories. When I ask them to pause at these physical reactions and explore what's happening, the tension in the body slowly changes."

Symptoms Disappear
Maartje: "In this way, we try to decouple the painful emotions from the unpleasant event. This causes the memory to lose its power and the event truly becomes part of the past. After the session, I often see a different child leave the room: relieved and much more relaxed." Nancy: "After the EMDR session, parents are tasked with closely monitoring what changes they notice regarding the symptoms and if there's any change in behavior. In the long term, we often see anxiety symptoms and behavioral problems disappear. In the past, it was often thought that young children wouldn't store impactful events, nowadays we know they do. If there's trauma, quick treatment is therefore desirable."

So, it's quite interesting, right? It's not clear however how it exactly works and there could also be downsides to it that we might be unaware of. But I'm seeing some pretty interesting changes so far. I'm currently reading a book on EMDR and will post any interesting findings here.

Imaginal Rescripting (IR)
Imaginal Rescripting is a CBT therapeutic technique that involves modifying a traumatic memory to create a more favorable outcome. In this approach, the person revisits the distressing memory and mentally alters the scenario to fulfill unmet needs or desires. This might involve imagining themselves confronting the perpetrator, asserting boundaries, or even engaging in retributive actions (such as setting them straight or even beating or chopping them up). Sometimes it's their current self who steps in and protects their former self within the memory.

If our efforts in the imaginal world has real life effects, who knows what kind of effects a therapy method such as this one can have, especially considering 'time' doesn't exist. I'll be following a course on it later this year to learn more about it.

Here's an example of a client who benefited from both EMDR and IR (I changed some personal details):

A woman experienced sexual abuse starting at age four, reportedly by multiple people over an extended period. As an adult, she developed coping mechanisms such as immersing herself in work. She had a pattern of entering abusive relationships, ultimately marrying a man who was emotionally distant and inconsiderate during intimacy. Throughout her life, she believed she was asexual,

Individuals who have experienced early sexual or physical abuse often develop avoidance behaviors, which can be both conscious and subconscious. These behaviors may include avoiding specific aspects of the traumatic events, certain emotions, or other related triggers. In this woman's case, it wasn't until she faced multiple challenging life events later on, such as the loss of a family member, that these suppressed traumatic experiences began to resurface. These events acted as triggers, causing her to become severely depressed. This crisis ultimately led her to seek professional help.

Throughout her life, she had undergone various forms of therapy, including some as a child and extensive body work as an adult, which provided some benefit. However, it was her engagement with trauma therapies, starting with EMDR, that allowed her to delve deeper. This process led to significant insights, helping her understand the motivations behind her life choices, recognize the impact of the abuse on her behavior and worldview, and gain a new perspective on her self-image.

Following her EMDR treatment, she underwent Imaginal Rescripting (IR) therapy. This approach was especially beneficial in helping her access and express anger, an emotion she'd previously struggled with. Through IR, she made a crucial realization: she was no longer helpless and the world around her wasn't threatening anymore. This shift in perspective was an important step in her healing process.

As therapy went on, she had the realization that she isn't asexual; rather, her traumatic experiences had distorted her perception of sexuality. She also gained a visceral understanding of the poor treatment she had endured from her husband. She also changed her view on work, no longer wanting to work herself to death. Instead, she wanted to take better care of herself and spend more time with others. She still has a way to go, but she's made great improvements.

Imaginal Exposure (IE)

Imaginal or Imagery Exposure is a therapeutic technique where individuals confront traumatic or distressing memories by vividly imagining and repeatedly recounting them, with the goal of reducing their emotional impact. Clients record their vivid retelling and listen to it multiple times daily. A person once told me it's like listening to a song over and over again until you get bored of it. A person can show a lot of emotional distress while imagining and retelling the story for the first time, but over time, they're able to recount it with more ease. During this process new insights and perspectives can be gained. Some people with severe traumas refrain from telling what happened to them in detail because they're afraid that they will "lose their minds", but through IE they discover they can tolerate revisiting the memory and that the memory itself can't cause further harm. As therapy progresses, they experience a decrease in the memory's emotional intensity, ultimately leading to improved emotional regulation and reduced symptoms.

Effectiveness of these therapies/techniques

All three therapies have been found to be effective in reducing PTSD-symptoms. With the help of DeepSeek, here are some examples from the literature:

EMDR
A meta-analysis by Bisson et al. (2013) found that EMDR is as effective as trauma-focused cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) for PTSD, with 70-90% of individuals experiencing significant symptom reduction after 3-15 sessions.
-Reference: Bisson, J. I., Roberts, N. P., Andrew, M., Cooper, R., & Lewis, C. (2013). Psychological therapies for chronic post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in adults. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, (12)

A study by Shapiro (2014) demonstrated that EMDR reduces PTSD symptoms by 50-70%, with some individuals achieving complete remission.
-Reference: Shapiro, F. (2014). The role of eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR) therapy in medicine: Addressing the psychological and physical symptoms stemming from adverse life experiences. The Permanente Journal, 18(1)

According to DeepSeek: "Across all conditions, EMDR has an average effectiveness rate of 50-80% for symptom reduction, with higher rates for PTSD and anxiety disorders."

Imaginal Rescripting
Studies show that Imaginal Rescripting reduces PTSD symptoms by 50-70%, with some individuals achieving complete remission.
Key Study:
Arntz et al. (2007) found that ImRs significantly reduced PTSD symptoms in patients with chronic PTSD, with many participants no longer meeting diagnostic criteria after treatment.
-Reference: Arntz, A., Tiesema, M., & Kindt, M. (2007). Treatment of PTSD: A comparison of imaginal exposure with and without imagery rescripting. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38(4), 345–370

Morina et al. (2017) write that "IR addresses complaints associated with aversive memories, is effective in treating a variety of mental health disorders, appears similarly effective as exposure, cognitive restructuring, or EMDR and that its treatment effects appear stable at 4–12 weeks following treatment".

Imaginal Exposure
Imaginal Exposure reduces PTSD symptoms by 50-70%, with many individuals achieving significant improvement or remission.
Foa et al. (2005) found that IE led to a 60-70% reduction in PTSD symptoms in patients with chronic PTSD.
-Reference: Foa, E. B., Hembree, E. A., Cahill, S. P., Rauch, S. A., Riggs, D. S., Feeny, N. C., & Yadin, E. (2005). Randomized trial of prolonged exposure for posttraumatic stress disorder with and without cognitive restructuring: Outcome at academic and community clinics. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(5), 953–964.

The benefits of Imaginal Exposure are often maintained over time, with many individuals reporting sustained improvement months or years after treatment.
Resick et al. (2012) found that IE led to long-term symptom reduction, with 50-60% of patients no longer meeting PTSD criteria at follow-up.
-Reference: Resick, P. A., Williams, L. F., Suvak, M. K., Monson, C. M., & Gradus, J. L. (2012). Long-term outcomes of cognitive-behavioral treatments for posttraumatic stress disorder among female rape survivors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 80(2), 201–210.
 
Last edited:
That is rather incredible, this made me think of something I read elsewhere about trauma processing, that is somewhat tied to eye movement and perhaps it's related to the process through which EMDR works.

I think it was the book Why We Sleep, the author speaks about one of the theoretical roles of dreaming and nightmares was to process emotionally intense experiences in a way that was safe and contained, while, recurring nightmares tend to become easier to manage as they become regular visitors.

And I daresay that for most intense events in our lives, this is effective enough to be processed during our sleep, however, more severe traumatic experiences may require the same method that REM sleep uses, only conscious.

The only other thing that comes to mind is how the body behaves, particularly the eyes, during traumatic events. I think it was Peter Levine who said, paraphrasing, that trauma is generated by an emotional response that goes not acted out after an intense or life threatening experience occurs. Not being able to fight back, or prevented from running, forced to remain immovable and so on. So, maybe what happens is that when in a traumatic event, our eyes, much like our bodies, freeze.

This freezing might be reenacted when contemplating those memories, if one is able to contemplate them at all, but if one moves one's eyes at least, one remains contemplating the experience while not frozen by it, releasing everything else little by little. Sort of like, challenging the "mesmerizing" effect that some memories have on one's consciousness by focusing attention elsewhere.

Thanks a lot for sharing, this is fascinating stuff.
 
Thanks for putting this information together Oxajil.

I've done Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing (EMDR) before and seems the most sound of the methods you described above. The therapy seems to bring up deeper psychological processing of the events, for example with the story of the woman recounting her abuse mother with the vacuum and the amount of tears and visible relaxation in the body. To me that is the biggest indication of the trauma therapy working: when the energies of crisis are processed and the patient begins to thaw out and feel more relaxed and embodied as a result.

I think it was in "The Body Keeps Score" that the author mentioned that one of the potential downsides of talk therapy is that a client could just be rehashing the same memory over and over again, causing them to dissociate further within themselves; this corresponds with a decrease in feeling about the event and more "3rd person" objectivity, causing the therapist to conclude that the therapy is helping the client, when it may in fact be doing the opposite by habituation re-traumatization.

I don't get the sense that's happening with EMDR, but I get some of those worries about Imaginal Rescripting (IR) and Imaginal Exposure (IE), depending on how it's conducted. Of course the devil is in the details. I think those could work quite well if a person is capable of being with themselves (i.e. having a coherent unity of sensory data, images, bodily sensations, affect/emotion, and meaning-making/narrative - given the acronym SIBAM by Peter Levine in his Somatic Experience trainings). But it seems like more preparation of the groundwork is needed to ensure they can contain those activated energies without dissociating (i.e. having their experience of the 5 elements of SIBAM uncoupling and disconnecting psychologically).

If our efforts in the imaginal world has real life effects, who knows what kind of effects a therapy method such as this one can have, especially considering 'time' doesn't exist. I'll be following a course on it later this year to learn more about it.

I think the ability of imaginal work to effect real world changes in ourselves and the world at large is directly tied into how grounded, emotionally self-attuned, and embodied we ourselves are. There are many people out there who suffer from chronic stress and all kinds of distressing personal histories who get told a lot of unhelpful things about manifestation and positive visualization. The creative powers for that kind of stuff, IMO, are in the unconscious, and when that is weighed down with all this other emotional noise and unconscious beliefs working at cross-purposes with the conscious intent, the results I think could be worse than useless.

I think the only caveat I see with EMDR is that it relies on having a solid piece of visual data to go by, which may not always be the case when it comes to pre-verbal trauma. It seems like you would have to find some triggering event they are aware of and reprocess that instead, and ideally tugging on that thread takes you into deeper waters over the lifespan of the therapeutic relationship.

In my own journey for healing trauma and in general just cultivating a more dynamic and adaptive autonomic nervous system, I found doing facilitated connected breathwork (of which bioenergetic breathing is a variety, minus facilitation) to be extremely effective also, providing there's the proper trauma-informed training and the like in facilitation. Similar to EMDR there is using a movement pattern (namely a breathing pattern) to build neuroplasticity to rewire things in the brain. Because of its bottom-up nature, there isn't really a need to get into the narratives of a client (although knowing their personal history does help a lot, for example if there's sexual abuse). It can also just train people to relax better, or be able to assume a high-energy state that feels safe and life-affirming, on account of rewiring the diaphragms of breathing to reinforce certain nervous system states of relaxation and activation.
 
I think it was in "The Body Keeps Score" that the author mentioned that one of the potential downsides of talk therapy is that a client could just be rehashing the same memory over and over again, causing them to dissociate further within themselves; this corresponds with a decrease in feeling about the event and more "3rd person" objectivity, causing the therapist to conclude that the therapy is helping the client, when it may in fact be doing the opposite by habituation re-traumatization.
Yes, I've heard this being mentioned several times on the forum, and it may be the case. However, if a person expresses their emotions, fears and thoughts during the process and comes to a different understanding of the event, I don't think that's dissociation but rather processing. People with traumas already show a lot of avoidance behavior with possible dissociation. By doing these therapies, the person is asked to 'take the bull by the horns'. Of course, it's important to keep in mind how far a person is willing to/can go at any given time (window of tolerance).
I think the ability of imaginal work to effect real world changes in ourselves and the world at large is directly tied into how grounded, emotionally self-attuned, and embodied we ourselves are. There are many people out there who suffer from chronic stress and all kinds of distressing personal histories who get told a lot of unhelpful things about manifestation and positive visualization. The creative powers for that kind of stuff, IMO, are in the unconscious, and when that is weighed down with all this other emotional noise and unconscious beliefs working at cross-purposes with the conscious intent, the results I think could be worse than useless.
It's not very clear to me what you're trying to say with the above.
I think the only caveat I see with EMDR is that it relies on having a solid piece of visual data to go by, which may not always be the case when it comes to pre-verbal trauma.
If there's no data at all about a pre-verbal traumatic event, perhaps it's possible that after uncovering pre-verbal trauma through hypnotic techniques, EMDR could be used to process and integrate those early experiences. However, once a pre-verbal traumatic event is uncovered, I'd first collect as much data as I can from people who were present or who could know more.

Also, sight isn't the only pathway to accessing memories or processing experiences. People with visual impairments, for example, can revisit past events by engaging their other senses such as hearing, touch, smell, or even proprioception (perception or awareness of the position and movement of the body). These non-visual sensory inputs can also be used to work through memories. However, more research is needed on this.
In my own journey for healing trauma and in general just cultivating a more dynamic and adaptive autonomic nervous system, I found doing facilitated connected breathwork (of which bioenergetic breathing is a variety, minus facilitation) to be extremely effective also, providing there's the proper trauma-informed training and the like in facilitation. Similar to EMDR there is using a movement pattern (namely a breathing pattern) to build neuroplasticity to rewire things in the brain. Because of its bottom-up nature, there isn't really a need to get into the narratives of a client (although knowing their personal history does help a lot, for example if there's sexual abuse). It can also just train people to relax better, or be able to assume a high-energy state that feels safe and life-affirming, on account of rewiring the diaphragms of breathing to reinforce certain nervous system states of relaxation and activation.
Yeah, I think the most effective treatment is a combination of different therapies and approaches. I agree that breathwork is also important, as we've seen with EE for example. :-) I also think bottom-up therapies are especially helpful for OPs, since they may lack the ability to achieve insight.
 
Thank you for opening this thread, very interesting stuff!

I've been doing a course (still doing it as it is very long) on trauma therapy that combines EMDR and Brainspotting with various other theoretical and technical approaches and so far I find it very thorough. The teacher is known for working with trauma and I myself have had therapy with him and was amazed at the results. In the course he also teaches Imaginal Rescripting and Parts work, so I wanted to add that to the list, not necessarily in the form of IFS precisely but just the concept of "parts" seems to be very effective for deidentification from internal states. It can be more or less imaginal and can work well with Rescripting.

I think that the idea of parts can really help a lot in the process because people tend to merge with internal states and just by adding that concept and bringing the awareness of different internal sensations, feelings, emotions, even thoughts, as "parts" can help people grow an internal "observer" and the experience of an "adult self" that is more grounded and is safe in the present moment. In my little experience so far I've found that just by exploring this in an imaginal way helps separate those states a bit and bring some perspective.

The downside of it, I think it's a bit related to what Whitecoast said, that some people may be prone to imagine "too much" and just lose focus. I think the problem with IFS (Internal Family Systems) is related to the fact that they stay too much in parts work and give too much importance to the parts and sometimes other approaches are needed too.

In this course we were taught that EMDR and Brainspotting go "straight to the point", while parts work is a bit softer and like a roundabout. That's what I noticed too while practising. A few people said something along the lines of "when we do the eyes thing I feel it all inside of me, when we do the parts thing it's like I see it all but I don't feel it as much inside of me".

Now, what I understand so far in trauma is that it is indeed necessary to have that association of feeling, memories, emotions, sensations, etc, because the main reason for being traumatized is dissociation, so we want to have an experience where we're not dissociated, and we want to increase our ability to remember that experience without feeling overwhelmed, and what is wonderful is that that helps desensitize the memory but also increases our resilience because it increases our ability to experience our present without feeling overwhelmed. So, one would think that doing something that dissociates the experience (deidentification via parts work) wouldn't be so good, but I think it depends.

Here I remember the concept of pendulation given by Peter Levine, to move back and forth so that we can increase the ability to experience distress fully without feeling overwhelmed by touching a bit of it and then going back to something more grounding, then going back to touch it a bit more, then going back to something more grounding... Here, overwhelm is just something that surpasses the person's ability to be with the experience without feeling completely overpowered by it, and that depends a lot on the person.

EMDR seems to do that in and on itself but I guess that for some people it helps to combine different things, especially at the beginning, so that the person can access distressing stuff with a bit more perspective and groundedness.

I think the only caveat I see with EMDR is that it relies on having a solid piece of visual data to go by, which may not always be the case when it comes to pre-verbal trauma. It seems like you would have to find some triggering event they are aware of and reprocess that instead, and ideally tugging on that thread takes you into deeper waters over the lifespan of the therapeutic relationship.

If there's no data at all about a pre-verbal traumatic event, perhaps it's possible that after uncovering pre-verbal trauma through hypnotic techniques, EMDR could be used to process and integrate those early experiences. However, once a pre-verbal traumatic event is uncovered, I'd first collect as much data as I can from people who were present or who could know more.

Yeah, things that are completely forgotten are more difficult and I think the approach that you suggest is pretty good, Oxajil. Brainspotting can help with that too as it is possible to work with just "sensations" even if there's no specific explicit memory associated with it, but ultimately, I think that being able to know what happened will be the most liberating if accompanied by some processing, as you suggest.

I just want to add a bit about Brainspotting, as I mentioned it in this post. It is a method developed by a person who did a lot of EMDR and experimented with doing it a bit differently. Instead of moving the eyes, he proposes that there are spots where people look which are like accessing points to memories and that by focusing on those spots people process stuff as well. It happens naturally too, as you may notice that when we talk about something, we might organically look to a spot and stay there, so it's just that, following the brain to the spot where the activation seems to be stronger and staying focused on it. I've pracitised with this method a bit and found it to be amazing as well. It's so fascinating how the eyes seem to be very important in these methods. As with EMDR, there isn't still a clear understanding of why this works, but there's something in it, just as there seems to be something to the method with colors mentioned by Persej here. Again, it's the eyes (with colors and light in that case).

Here's a summary given by Deepseek about Brainspotting:

Brainspotting therapy is a relatively new, brain-based approach to trauma treatment developed by David Grand in 2003. It is based on the idea that trauma and emotional distress can become "stuck" in the brain and body, leading to symptoms like anxiety, depression, or PTSD. During a session, the therapist helps the client identify "brainspots"—specific eye positions that correlate with the activation of traumatic memories or emotions. These spots are thought to access deeper, subcortical areas of the brain where trauma is stored.

Once a brainspot is located, the client focuses on it while simultaneously paying attention to their internal experiences, such as bodily sensations, emotions, or thoughts. This dual awareness allows the brain to naturally process and release the trapped trauma, often without the need for extensive talking or retelling of the traumatic event. Brainspotting is considered a gentle, client-centered approach that leverages the brain's innate capacity for healing, making it effective for a wide range of emotional and psychological issues.

I asked it to summarize in a paragraph something about its effectiveness and here it is:

Brainspotting shows promise in treating trauma, PTSD, anxiety, and somatic symptoms, with many clients and therapists reporting positive outcomes. However, research is still limited, with small sample sizes and few high-quality studies. While anecdotal evidence and clinical reports are encouraging, more rigorous research is needed to fully validate its effectiveness and mechanisms. It is often used as a complementary or alternative therapy, particularly for those who don’t respond to traditional treatments.
 
I think imaginal work is fascinating. In effect, reading the romance novels is a healing process in and of itself as the outcome for the program/wound is essentially changed in an imaginal world, and you go deeply through the experience through your emotions.

I visited Lourdes not long ago and was genuinely impressed by the positive energy in that grotto and the multiple messages of gratefulness and devotion from people all over the world. The grounding effect in my solar plexus felt like the time I visited Stonehenge and Newgrange. The Cs about Lourdes:

Q: (L) I have been reading recently about the shrine at Lourdes where the Virgin Mary supposedly appeared to Bernadette Soubirous...

A: Energy focusing center.

Q: (L) What kind of energy is focused there?

A: Positive due to consistent prayer patternings.

Q: (L) Okay, what appeared to Bernadette?

A: Imaging energy consciousness wave.

Q:
(L) Was this image out of her own mind?

A: Close.

Q: (L) The healings that take place...

A: Because of the concentration of positive energy.
 
I think that the idea of parts can really help a lot in the process because people tend to merge with internal states and just by adding that concept and bringing the awareness of different internal sensations, feelings, emotions, even thoughts, as "parts" can help people grow an internal "observer" and the experience of an "adult self" that is more grounded and is safe in the present moment. In my little experience so far I've found that just by exploring this in an imaginal way helps separate those states a bit and bring some perspective.

The downside of it, I think it's a bit related to what Whitecoast said, that some people may be prone to imagine "too much" and just lose focus. I think the problem with IFS (Internal Family Systems) is related to the fact that they stay too much in parts work and give too much importance to the parts and sometimes other approaches are needed too.
I couldn't agree more. My psychologist gave me the task to look at my part/I that gets easily overwhelmed by life's demands and what it is telling me and why it is there and I find that I can't do it. Or I don't want to do it, as it won't be helpful, which could be my intuition talking?

Interestingly, learning more about coaching which is focused on the present and the future and steers clear from the past I feel as if I have been given the tools to deal with my feelings of overwhelm by chunking down my aims and making them feasible. As in: baby-steps. Learning more about neuroplasticity and how we can build new habits that stick has been very helpful, too.
Brainspotting can help with that too as it is possible to work with just "sensations" even if there's no specific explicit memory associated with it, but ultimately, I think that being able to know what happened will be the most liberating if accompanied by some processing, as you suggest.

I just want to add a bit about Brainspotting, as I mentioned it in this post. It is a method developed by a person who did a lot of EMDR and experimented with doing it a bit differently. Instead of moving the eyes, he proposes that there are spots where people look which are like accessing points to memories and that by focusing on those spots people process stuff as well. It happens naturally too, as you may notice that when we talk about something, we might organically look to a spot and stay there, so it's just that, following the brain to the spot where the activation seems to be stronger and staying focused on it. I've pracitised with this method a bit and found it to be amazing as well. It's so fascinating how the eyes seem to be very important in these methods.
I had one or two Brainspotting sessions and found that by looking at a certain angle and staying there I could see a traumatic scene unfold before my eyes, it was information that had only made itself known in my dreams. I did start to dissociate when I kept looking at the scene, but found that after the session was over emotions and images were able to come up without my dissociating, which means as Oxajil said above that I was processing.

Curiously, during a Family Constellations session I looked at the same angle and again a traumatic scene unfolded before my eyes, but that was not the counsellor's intention! It happened spontaneously. I have also found that by looking at another angle images can come up as well.

In my case I can't check with family members whether the information is correct or not and I am not certain whether they would tell me so. If we assume that body or implicit memories are correct or that some information is hidden in our thoughts, dreams and habits we may get more information? And even if the information is more symbolic of certain traumas I find that I can still process pain, grief and feelings of terror just by allowing them to come up. That's my thinking anyway at this point.
I think imaginal work is fascinating. In effect, reading the romance novels is a healing process in and of itself as the outcome for the program/wound is essentially changed in an imaginal world, and you go deeply through the experience through your emotions.
Meanwhile, I continue reading our romance novels...:-) There is so much more I understand about myself and others while reading these super healing books, although my processing certain events seem to happen in a subtle and gentle manner and I can't really put my finger on it, but maybe that's just me.
 
Meanwhile, I continue reading our romance novels...:-) There is so much more I understand about myself and others while reading these super healing books, although my processing certain events seem to happen in a subtle and gentle manner and I can't really put my finger on it, but maybe that's just me.
And it's a processing that you're actually looking forward to completing, I think that's the brilliance of the romance novels, because of the way the stories are structured, you're curiosity is peaked and you are actually looking to get engaged with the story fully, and the wounds, trauma and their resolution.
 
If our efforts in the imaginal world has real life effects, who knows what kind of effects a therapy method such as this one can have, especially considering 'time' doesn't exist. I'll be following a course on it later this year to learn more about it.

I found the following from Harville Hendrix's book Keeping the Love You Find to be a good summary of the timelessness of emotions.


NEW MIND, OLD BRAIN

It is our grand, highly evolved brain that separates us upright humanoids from the lower orders. We like to think of our brain as that part of us which digests, organizes, and analyzes the mountains of diverse information we take in. We use our brain to plan, strategize, invent, create, make decisions. It's not surprising that we identify with the brain—we see it as who we are, our consciousness, our center. Well, that is true, as far as it goes. The cerebral cortex— which I call the "new" brain—does do all those wonderful things. But that is, literally, only the half of it. Beneath the convoluted dome familiar from anatomical charts is what I term the "old" brain, which has a profound impact on our behavior in relationships, and that's quite a different story.

There are two parts to the old brain. The brain stem, sometimes referred to as the "reptilian" brain (which shows you how far back it goes), is the brain's primitive inner core. It controls vital physical systems, from reproduction and sleep to blood circulation and muscle response. It's where the action is. Cradling the brain stem is the limbic system, a mammalian addition, the seat of primitive, powerful emotions linking the autonomous functions of the brain stem and the cerebral cortex. The old brain—the brainstem and limbic system—is our survival brain.

Unlike the new brain, which takes in and processes the data of the external world, the instinctual old brain is aware of what's beyond its boundaries only through sensation and feeling. Simple and primitive, it makes broad distinctions related to its safety and survival mainly via the images, symbols, and thoughts relayed to it via the new brain. Eternally concerned with survival, the old brain recognizes certain patterns that it has learned to associate with "love," "loss," "anger," or "danger." It is unable to make subtle distinctions according to circumstance; its knee-jerk reactions are deeply ingrained, and blown way out of proportion to the stimulus. All threats are life threatening; the slightest frown signals total rejection.

Most important to understand about the old brain is that it exists outside of time: it makes no distinctions as to why or where or when something happened. Your old brain cannot tell the difference between the stimulus it received thirty years ago when your mother didn't come when you cried and the one it received today when your partner called from work an hour late, canceling your date for the evening. As a baby you may have responded to feeling abandoned by sucking your thumb and cooing to yourself; today you respond to the situation by eating a dozen cookies and, tuning out your anxiety, watching some mindless TV movie until your partner makes amends. In both cases the feelings, and the response, are the same. The response is instinctual; the old brain is doing what it has been conditioned to do in order to survive.

It's easy to believe, when we live in high-rises, drive fast cars, buy our food and clothing ready-made, and "choose" who we will marry, that we are masters of our primal, instinctual natures. We are fooling ourselves, however rational or intellectual we may think we are, however finely tuned our cerebral cortexes. The old and new brain interact in a complex system of checks and balances, a dance of instinct and emotion tempering intellect and reason. We are only human, after all. The new brain cannot hope to overpower the old brain; it is its servant.

Be we humans are unique in our self-awareness: we know that we think. Our forebrain, part of the "new" brain, is conscious of the contents of the cortex. It is the means by which we observe ourselves (and even observe ourselves observing ourselves), enabling us to see the interplay of images, thoughts, and symbols. It is the medium of self-change through which we can acknowledge the power of the old brain and cooperate with it.

Interesting side note, he goes on to say that the 'romantic timelessness' one feels when meeting a toxic partner during the love-bombing phase is actually a rekindling of the infant's love for/attachment to a toxic caregiver. This happens because the patterns of behaviour (or maybe FRV) of the new partner seems similar enough to that of the parents. So the old brain recognizes the possibility of finding the love that was missing, and responds with survival-level attachment to the new toxic relationship. It's like trying to solve the problems of the lack of love in the past, or learn the still unlearned lessons there. Something like that.

Anyways, with regards to the question of why a therapy like EMDR works, or why the eyes seem to hold a key to our ability to process trauma, I read a book by Francesca McCartney called Body of Health that might provide the beginnings of an answer. She writes about the relationship amino acid productions and our emotional state in a way that is similar to Laura's writings about neuropeptides in The Wave. What McCartney adds, however, is that the function of light entering the eyes, and the hypothalamus play a large role.

"NO TIME" AND THE NOW

Your pathway to Now is a practice of meditation. In meditation, you can verify the existence of the Now with your intuitive, sensitive body.

Within your head, you can also locate "the power of Now" in the area of your hypothalamus gland. Neurochemist Candace Pert, PhD, has researched and tested this temporal portal and found it to be an organ of intuition. For many years, I theorized, based both on direct experience and years of clinical observation, that the hypothalamus is the "motherboard" of intuition in the body.

It is also noteworthy that the hypothalamus relates to emotional addiction, and addictive emotions remove a person from the spiritual realm and the Now. The movie What the Bleep Do We Know?! offers an explanation of the tendency to cling to unhealthy mental attitudes. For example, in the mental attitude of the "victim personality," the hypothalamus produces "victim" amino acids that flood the body, carrying bundles of past victim-story emotions.

If a person stops thinking and feeling habitual victim-oriented thoughts, the hypothalamus stops putting out these amino acids, and the person goes into withdrawal. this is why it can be so difficult to let go of painful memories; it takes a lot of work!

If you have trouble releasing victimhood or painful memories, there is hope. During your meditation sanctuary practice, you can flood your body with healthy emotions by repeating positive affirmations. In time, this will hardwire a healthier emotional reality into the body. This is a powerful practice for creating a body of health.

The hypothalamus is more than just a delivery system for emotions; we have inner and outer sight because of it. When light enters the eye, it travels to the vision centers of the brain and to the hypothalamus, which controls almost all of the functions of the pituitary, which in turn affects all the endocrine glands. This means the hypothalamus is a major collecting and controlling station for information from the external and internal environment.

The hypothalamus consists of concentrated bundles of neuronal fibres within a vacuum. These neurons act as transducers - devices that are actuated by power from one system, and supply power, usually in another form, to a second system - that use quantum electromagnetic effects depending on the polarizing ability of the vaccuum. The vacuum itself becomes a transducer into physical state-energies for consciousness-energy in nonphysical states. The vacuum is the physically effective yet non-physical transition state that leads to and from the domains of consciousness. This implies that the hypothalamus is a bioenergetic mechanism for dissolving time-concepts in order to explore timeless metadimensions.

Or maybe it could be said that the hypothalamus is the region of the brain most responsible for accessing the imaginal realm? This would seem to be an important addendum to Hendrix's claim above, that it's the new brain that interprets thoughts, symbols, and images - or the language of the imaginal realm. I don't really know how to square the two. The brain sure is complex.

Energy healer Barbara Brennan, author of Hands of Light, teaches a "high sense perception" program that includes visualizing a meditation spot that she describes as a beautiful point of light in the centre of the head that grows into a brilliant ball of light. She clairvoyantly sees this light as being located in the root area of the crown and third-eye chakras, where the pituitary and pineal glands are located. As my realtor friends say, "Location is everything." And indeed, in order to develop your intuition pragmatically and contemplatively, you must bring your awareness into your meditation sanctuary, that private, sacred space within yourself that is an entryway into the experience of Now.

[...]

Perception is a creative force, empowered through visualization, knowing, and intention. Visualization is a way to use imagination to gain intuitive insight, with imagery serving as the language of intuition. Knowing provides the insight to see through the filtering screen of thoughts, images, and feelings to the formless context of knowledge. Intention is a deliberate decision to create. It is premeditated; it is planned. Intention is not a fleeting thought. It is not a wish, hope, or worry. It is a decision, made with purpose, to produce something. It is a force unto itself. When your awareness is focused on your hypothalamus, you are operating from a place of fluidity, effectiveness, and calm assurance. You are truly smarter because you generate a state of mind in which your intellectual analysis of a situation is combined with acute sensory perception and creative intuition.

All that said, there is something to the old saying that the eyes are the windows to the Soul.
 
Thank you all for your interesting comments! Here's an explanation of what imagery or imaginal rescripting is, its history, its effectivity, and its possible mechanism from the book Imagery Rescripting by Remco van der Wijngaart (bolding by me):

What is imagery rescripting?

Imagery rescripting (IR) is a therapeutic technique that addresses the content of events in our lives that are stored in our memory, and cause difficulties. In short, during IR, an unpleasant or traumatic event (the mental representation of that event) is retrieved from our memory and, through imagery, the course of the event is changed in a more desired direction. A recent meta-analysis shows that strong therapeutic effects are achieved with this technique (Morina et al., 2017). Interest in and the application of IR has increased considerably since the 1990s (Arntz, 2012). The literature describes two different ways in which this change in the course of events can be realized.

Variant 1: imagery rescripting in combination with cognitive restructuring

In this variant, IR is preceded by a discussion in which the meaning of the image is examined and adjusted. During rescripting, the more realistic conclusions of that discussion are fed into the imagery with questions such as 'What do you now know about (the likelihood of this threat/what alternative interpretations could be)?' (Grey et al., 2002). The change in the course of events sometimes takes place on the basis of alternative scenarios discussed beforehand. IR can be a repeated exercise of the alternative scenario devised in advance (see e.g. Hackmann, 1998).

Variant 2: imagery rescripting without prior cognitive restructuring

In this variant of IR, the course of events is changed during the imagery without prior cognitive restructuring. Rescripting is then done on the basis of questions such as, 'What do you think of this situation?', and 'What do you need now?' (Arntz, 2015). In this variant, therefore, the course of the visualized events is not determined on the basis of a scenario discussed beforehand, but each IR intervention can each time lead to a different course of events.

In this book, imagery rescripting is described as a standalone intervention, without the addition of other treatment elements such as cognitive restructuring. Therefore, this book will mainly describe the second variant of IR (Arntz, 2015; Arntz & Van Genderen, 2010; Arntz & Weertman, 1999).

The possibilities for imaginal rescripting of the course of unpleasant experiences are endless: aggressors can be defeated; enemies humiliated; victims can be saved, comforted and treated with compassion; or there can be communication with the deceased. The rescripting regularly includes elements that are factually impossible, such as the deployment of superhuman forces or supernatural beings, or the execution of impossible changes of reality (see, for example, Arntz, 2015).

The superficial impression could emerge that the application of IR is merely a change from a negative image from the past to a positive image. The real purpose of imagery rescripting is to allow the client to develop a new view of past events, to generate new feelings that do not necessarily have to be positive (such as anger), learn to recognize non-validated basic needs again, or to face reality (e.g. experiences of abuse) so that a grieving process can begin (Holmes, 2007).

Which images are targeted using imagery rescripting?

With IR, intrusive and unpleasant images can be processed, such as traumatic memories (Arntz et al., 2007; Grunert et al., 2007), or fantasy images of negative events without clear autobiographical memories (for example, the image of stabbing someone for clients with obsessive-compulsive disorder or the image of a suicide in depression). However, IR can also focus on meaningful, but not necessarily traumatic, memories that guide underlying schematic beliefs such as borderline and other personality disorders (Arntz et al., 2007; Holmes et al., 2007; Weertman & Arntz, 2007). For example, this could be a reminder of a situation in which the client was not listened to, making her feel unimportant and lonely.

The history of imagery rescripting

Although interest in IR has grown strongly since the 1990s, the technique itself has been used much longer. As early as 1889, Pierre Janet described examples of guided imagery exercises, in which he had the client rewrite memories (see Van der Hart et al., 1989). However, his work was ignored in the century that followed, which was dominated by the Freudian and post-Freudian psychoanalytic approach (Edwards, 2007).

However, in 1970 Beck described the use of simple imagery techniques, and Freeman (1981) described how the use of dream images could make an important contribution to a client's cognitive case conceptualization. Structured imagery exercises were developed and became known within behavioural therapy in the 1970s and 1980s – for example, in the form of systematic desensitization, or counterconditioning (Arntz, 2012; Edwards, 2007).

However, in the 1980s there still seemed to be a strong separation between the more sceptical, academic approach to IR on the one hand, and clinical practice on the other, where various experiential techniques were already used frequently. For example, Erskine and Moursund (1988) described the use of what would now be called IR in the healing of ‘scripts’ (a term from transactional analysis that could be considered synonymous with core beliefs or schemas in Young’s model (Young et al., 2003)). Young began to integrate techniques from transactional analysis into schema-focused cognitive behavioural therapy (Young, 1990). In 1995, a first publication appeared on imagery rescripting as we know it today (Smucker et al., 1995).

Over the past few years, scientific interest in IR has increased enormously (Hackmann et al., 2011) and IR has been integrated into various well-tested treatment protocols, such as cognitive behavioural therapy for PTSD, social anxiety disorder (Clark et al., 2006; Ehlers & Clark, 2000; Ehlers et al., 2005), and nightmare disorder (Davis & Wright, 2007; Krakow et al., 2001) and cognitive therapy and schema therapy for personality disorders (Arntz & Van Genderen, 2009; Giesen-Bloo et al., 2006; Layden et al., 1993; Young et al., 2003).

The effectiveness of imagery rescripting

A recent meta-analysis by Morina and colleagues (2017) describes 19 studies on the effectiveness of IR in a total of 363 patients. IR proved effective for the treatment of unpleasant memories in various disorders, such as posttraumatic stress disorder, depressive disorder, social anxiety disorder, body dysmorphic disorder, bulimia nervosa, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. IR had a positive effect, and this effect was achieved within an average of 4.5 sessions (Morina et al., 2017).

Below is an overview of the various disorders in which IR appears to be a useful intervention. For each disorder, a summary of the evidence for the role of imagery in the disorder is provided. Next, results regarding the effectiveness of IR in treating the disorder are summarized. It is not our intention to provide an exhaustive overview of published studies. However, we did try to list the key results.

Post-traumatic stress disorder
Intrusive symptoms such as recurring re-experiences of, or dreams about, traumatic event(s) are characteristic of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD; APA, 2013). Research has shown that PTSD, regardless of the type of trauma, mainly involves visual images, followed by other sensory experiences, such as physical sensations, sounds, or taste sensations (Ehlers et al., 2004). The mental images are often meaningful fragments of the traumatic memory. However, these images do not always have to be a faithful representation of what happened during the traumatic event. The images can also be distorted and, above all, reflect the subjective meaning that the event had for the person at the time, or has had since (Hackmann, 2011).

For example, someone may have intrusive images relating to a car accident she had in the past. At the time of the accident, she thought she would not survive. This experience keeps coming back in the form of imagery in which she sees herself dying in the car accident. The images are experienced as something that happens now, rather than realising that it concerns a memory of something from the past. As a result, the images are often linked to a sense of imminent danger (Ehlers & Clark, 2000), despite information from the patient’s context that this is not the case. In addition to fear or helplessness, images can also induce anger, disgust, sadness, shame, or feelings of guilt (Hackmann, 2011).

Imagery rescripting for PTSD
Ehlers and Clark integrated IR in their cognitive therapy for PTSD in the 1990s and achieved strong effects with this combination therapy (Ehlers & Clark, 2000; Ehlers et al., 2005; Smucker & Niederee, 1995). However, the effect of IR has not been studied separately. The aforementioned meta-analysis by Morina and colleagues (2017) did examine the effectiveness of IR in various disorders. Out of 19 included studies, eight concerned the treatment of PTSD (Morina et al., 2017). In three of these studies, IR was compared with another condition, namely a waiting list condition (Jung & Steil, 2013), imaginal exposure (Øktedalen et al., 2015) or EMDR (Alliger-Horn et al., 2015).

IR demonstrated to be an effective treatment of PTSD symptoms in these three studies (Morina et al., 2017). Not only were treatments consisting of ten or more sessions effective (see, for example, Kindt et al., 2007), but two or three sessions appeared to reduce PTSD symptoms (Alliger-Horn et al., 2015; Jung & Steil, 2013; Steil et al., 2011). IR also seems to reduce difficulties other than anxiety, such as feelings of guilt, anger, or shame (Grunert et al., 2007; Øktedalen et al., 2015). Furthermore, the effect of IR was found to increase when the rescripting took place early in the traumatic image, preventing the actual traumatic event and providing early safety. Hence, it does not seem necessary to relive the trauma in detail during treatment (Arntz et al., 2013). Raabe and colleagues (2015) also found that, as with imaginal exposure, IR was also effective for the treatment of PTSD as a result of childhood abuse, even without a prior stabilization phase.

Social anxiety disorder
Research has shown that a distorted, negative self-image is a very common condition in social anxiety disorder (see, for example, Moscovitch et al., 2011; Reimer & Moscovitch, 2015; Stopa, 2009). For example, a patient described the image she had formed of an approaching job interview: she started to cough heavily, as a result of which she could no longer utter a meaningful word, she turned increasingly red, and eventually had to leave the scene in frustration.

In cognitive models of social anxiety disorder, such negative self-images are seen as a sustaining factor (Clark & Wells, 1995; Rapee & Heimberg, 1997). A negative self-image makes people more anxious, incites them to use more safety behaviours, and is related to the belief that they are less competent and that they are actually assessed as socially less competent by independent assessors (Wild et al., 2007). These images are often related to memories of unpleasant events underlying the onset or worsening of the disorder, such as being bullied, humiliated, or criticized (Hackmann et al., 2000; Moscovitch et al., 2011; Wild et al., 2007, 2008).

Imagery rescripting for social anxiety disorder
A first controlled study showed that one session of IR of unpleasant memories of social situations in the past led to a significant change in the meaning of the memories and the reported social anxiety difficulties, whereas in the control condition, in which memories were only verbally processed, no changes occurred (Wild et al., 2008). These effects sustained a week after the session. In this study, IR was combined with cognitive restructuring. In a later controlled study, this combination also proved to be superior to the placebo control condition in which the participant’s problems were listened to with understanding (Lee & Kwon, 2013). This effect was also maintained at three months follow-up. IR as a standalone treatment of social anxiety i.e. without cognitive restructuring, may also be effective (Nillson et al., 2012). Although this study involved a small number of participants, the importance of cognitive restructuring appears less important than initially thought by the authors.

Furthermore, a study by Reimer and Moscovitch (2015) showed that a single session of IR not only led to a reduction in social anxiety symptoms, but also to a reduction in the emotional intensity of the autobiographical memories related to the onset of the symptoms. Participants also reported a reduction in negative thoughts about themselves or others, and reduced feelings of shame. On the other hand, feelings of satisfaction and pride about their rescripted autobiographical memories increased. Perhaps the most intriguing fact from the studies mentioned above is that only one session of IR was sufficient to produce the effects. However, it should be noted that although many of the participants suffered from social anxiety, they did not suffer from this fear to the degree that they sought help themselves. Frets et al. (2014) did study patients who had signed up with a long-term, persistent social anxiety disorder, often also involving comorbidity (N=6). In this study, the treatment was not limited to a fixed number of sessions; it could last as long as the therapist and patient felt was necessary. This resulted in treatments ranging from 5 to 17 sessions, with an average of 11.2 sessions.

This study also showed that without prior cognitive restructuring, IR can be an effective intervention in the treatment of social anxiety disorder, with effects that were maintained for at least six months after treatment. Norton and Abbott (2016) studied the relative effect of both cognitive restructuring and IR compared to a non-intervention control condition. Their study, including 60 participants, showed that one IR session can be effective for the treatment of social anxiety disorder. IR and cognitive restructuring were both effective, but on different outcome measures. The authors indicated that several participants were so overwhelmed by fear, shame, or powerlessness during the imagery exercise, that they were unable to independently rescript the image. For these participants it might be necessary for the therapist to do the rescripting first, in preparation for the rescripting by the patients themselves. In conclusion, there is increasing evidence that IR can be a standalone and potentially effective treatment for social anxiety disorder. IR seems to reduce not only social anxiety difficulties, but also feelings of shame and the credibility of negative core beliefs about oneself or others. These effects seem to persist for at least three to six months after treatment.

Specific phobias

There is strong evidence that imagery and visual images play an important role in the maintenance and perhaps also in the development of specific phobias. For example, patients with arachnophobia created frightening and distorted images in which the spider suddenly became larger, or in which they were held by a spider larger than themselves (Arntz et al., 1993). Hunt et al. (2006) found that 78% of people with a specific phobia reported visual images that were fear-related. Some of these images were distorted and highly improbable (such as the image that a rat would bite into the foot of the patient and not let go, whatever they did (Hunt et al., 2006)). These findings suggest that imagery is an important part of specific phobia and that these mental images can contribute to the persistence of the phobic fear. Other research has shown that imagery can play a role in the onset of specific phobia. For example, children were able to develop new fears when they were shown images of non-existent beings, accompanied by a vivid, negative description of these beings (Field, 2006; Field & Lawson, 2003).

Imagery rescripting for a specific phobia
Hunt and colleagues (2006) examined the relative effectiveness of IR combined with cognitive restructuring (CR) in the treatment of 60 participants suffering from a fear of snakes. IR with CR turned out to be more effective than exposure in vivo for high-anxiety participants. Exposure in vivo, on the other hand, turned out to be more effective for the low-anxiety participants. An important finding was that participants found IR with CR less aversive than exposure in vivo. A second study showed that IR with CR is at least as effective as exposure in vivo for the treatment of a specific phobia (Hunt & Fenton, 2007). In conclusion, IR seems to be an effective treatment for specific phobias.

Obsessive-compulsive disorder
Obsessive-compulsive disorder, along with PTSD, is one of the disorders characterized by recurring, intrusive images (APA, 2013). Systematic studies showed that 81-95% of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder reported vivid and disturbing images (Lipton et al., 2010; Speckens et al., 2007). The disorder is distinguished from other anxiety disorders by a higher frequency of images that are less related to autobiographical memories. The content of the images also differs and is more often about danger in the present (e.g. becoming infected with germs) or future danger (e.g. killing someone) (Lipton et al., 2010; Rachmann, 2007; Speckens et al., 2007). In the majority of cases, these images were experienced from the first-person perspective (Lipton et al., 2010).

Imagery rescripting for obsessive-compulsive disorder
Although the intrusive images are not a direct representation of autobiographical memories, they often appear to be thematically related to memories of unpleasant events that preceded the onset of the disorder (Veale et al., 2015). Processing these memories could therefore have a therapeutic effect on obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Veale and colleagues (2015) showed that a single session of IR resulted in clinically significant symptom reduction in seven out of 12 participants, and the authors suggest that multiple sessions of IR could be even more effective. Maloney and colleagues (2019) offered the opportunity to adjust the number of sessions of IR to what was necessary to produce a significant symptom improvement. This study included 13 patients who experienced insufficient effects from previous exposure with response prevention treatment. Six patients had significantly fewer difficulties after just one session of IR. For the other patients, on average two sessions were needed to achieve that result. These effects were largely retained after a period of three months. In summary, research on IR in obsessive-compulsive disorder is limited, but the first small studies show encouraging, although preliminary, results.

Depression
In groups of depressed patients, 44-96% reported having intrusive, negative images of unpleasant autobiographical memories (Brewin et al., 1996; Newby & Moulds, 2011; Patel et al., 2007). These images can relate, for example, to physical or sexual abuse in the past, humiliation during youth (for example, being bullied at school), experiences of failure (for example, being fired) or overwhelming grief (for example, the loss of a loved one) (see for a review: Weßlau & Steep, 2014). Not only are depressed patients characterized – compared to non-depressed people – by a higher number of negative images, but also by a lower number of positive images (Holmes et al, 2008; Morina et al., 2011; Moscovitch et al., 2011; Pile & Lau, 2018). Depressive patients, just like those with an anxiety disorder, also appear to have more intrusive images about negative events in the future, the so-called flash-forwards (Morina et al., 2011). Examples are images in which patients see themselves automutilating, committing suicide or seeing their own funeral, or the consequences of their death (Crane et al., 2012; Holmes et al., 2007).

Imagery rescripting for depression
Compared to the more verbal cognitive behavioural therapies, imagery has received less attention as part of the treatment of depression (Holmes et al., 2016). Hence, there are relatively few studies that have investigated the effect of IR in depression. Brewin and colleagues (2009) treated 10 patients suffering from depression, of whom some showed severe, chronic depressive symptoms, with an average of eight sessions of IR focused on intrusive memories. The majority of the patients showed a clinically significant improvement in symptoms and the average symptom decrease corresponded to that produced in a standard cognitive behavioural therapy of about 16 sessions. This effect was maintained for at least one year after completion of treatment.

Moritz and colleagues (2018) investigated whether IR can be effective as a self-help technique for the treatment of depression, using a comprehensive and less extensive patient manual explaining the intervention and exercises. Compared to a waiting list condition, patients in the IR condition where the extensive handbook was used had significantly fewer depressive symptoms. However, it turned out to be mainly patients with more serious difficulties, more confidence in the technique, and a greater willingness to change who benefitted from the treatment.

In addition to IR for autobiographical memories, one could also focus on future-oriented images of suicide or self-mutilation. These images may function as short-term emotion regulation (Selby et al., 2007). However, in the longer term these future-oriented images seem to increase the likelihood of actual suicidal behaviour (Crane et al., 2012). The effect of IR on these forward-looking images has not yet been investigated. In summary, there is preliminary evidence that IR might be a good alternative treatment strategy for depression. IR as a standalone treatment or as part of a cognitive behavioural therapy seems to reduce depressive symptoms (Brewin et al., 2009). However, more research is needed to determine whether IR is actually an effective treatment for depression.

Eating disorders
There are indications that mental images contribute to the persistence of eating disorders (e.g. Cooper, 2011). For example, patients with bulimia nervosa report more negative mental images than people without an eating disorder (Somerville et al., 2007). These negative images are related to food, body weight, and body shapes (Somerville et al., 2007), or – prior to vomiting – to social rejection (Hinrichsen et al., 2007). However, Dugué and colleagues (2016) found that patients with an eating disorder (81% binge eating disorder and 19% bulimia nervosa) did not report more images of body shape and weight than the control group containing healthy participants and patients suffering from non-eating disorders – although these images were associated with a greater urge to eat. Recurrent and vivid, negative self-images are also reported in anorexia nervosa (Cooper et al., 2007a).

However, patients with anorexia nervosa do not seem to experience more visual images of body shape and weight than people without this disorder. But they do report a larger number of images that are associated with specific auditory experiences, such as hearing negative comments (e.g. that they should become thinner, that they should vomit or that they are fat), with related feelings of shame, sadness, and fear (Cooper et al., 2007a). Images reported by patients suffering from eating disorders are frequently related to specific autobiographical memories. In bulimia nervosa, these are often reminders of negative comments about their weight, body shape, and appearance (Somerville et al., 2007) and to humiliation, abuse, or abandonment (Hinrichsen et al., 2007). The early recollections of these eating disorder-related experiences date from the period when one was on average about ten years old (Cooper et al., 2007a; Somerville et al., 2007). In anorexia nervosa, often memories related to rejection and worthlessness are reported (Cooper et al., 2007a).

Imagery rescripting for eating disorders
Only limited research has examined the effectiveness of IR in eating disorders. In 2007, Cooper and colleagues compared the effect of one session of IR with a control intervention session (verbal review of image related to eating disorder) in 24 patients with bulimia nervosa. IR led to less belief in eating disorder-related beliefs, a better mood, and a reduced urge to eat (Cooper et al., 2007b). Dugué and colleagues (2019) were the first to conduct a controlled study comparing IR with cognitive restructuring, in 36 patients with bulimia nervosa or binge eating disorder. IR was not superior to cognitive restructuring, although the effect of IR on core beliefs was larger at follow-up. However, the study was limited in scope and there was a large drop-out at one week follow-up, so future studies must reveal whether IR is a good alternative treatment of bulimia nervosa or binge eating disorder.

Another study compared the effect of IR with a cognitive intervention (cognitive dissonance) – both offered online – in women who were not satisfied with their bodies and were at risk of developing an eating disorder (Pennesi & Wade, 2018). The results give a first indication that this online variant of IR can lead to greater acceptance of one’s body image and to more self-compassion. In summary, mental images relating to body, body shape and eating seem relevant to the development and persistence of eating disorders. There is a clear relationship between such images and autobiographical memories that are associated with the onset or worsening of the eating disorder. However, research into the effect of IR in eating disorders is still in its infancy. The first, preliminary results are positive, but more research is needed.

Nightmares

A nightmare disorder is characterized by frequent nightmares about insecurity or threats to physical or emotional integrity. The nightmares are accompanied by a strong fear, anger, sadness, and disgust (APA, 2013). The nightmares are usually well remembered after waking up and cause great suffering (Lancee & Schrijnemaekers, 2013; Spoormaker et al., 2006). Nightmares are common: 83% of adults report one or more nightmares a year (Hublin et al., 1999; Schredl, 2016) and 2-5 % say they have one or more nightmares a week. The nightmare disorder is common in psychiatric populations (Nielsen & Levin, 2007; Schredl, 2016; Spoormaker et al., 2006). Patients with a nightmare disorder experience a high degree of powerlessness and uncontrollability with regard to their nightmares (Kunze et al., 2019). IR offers the possibility of changing the storyline in nightmares and this can lead to the experience of being able to do something about the content of the nightmares (Kunze et al., 2016). IR is an intervention par excellence for this disorder.

Imagery rescripting for nightmares
For many years, nightmares were treated using imagery rehearsal therapy. For a detailed description of this intervention, please see publications by Krakow and Zadra (2006, 2010). In short, this treatment is comprised of two components: a cognitive restructuring of thoughts about (having) the nightmares and a second component in which the storyline of the nightmare is rescripted, after which this new scenario is repeated daily in one’s mind.

Imagery rehearsal therapy was effective in reducing the frequency of nightmares and related suffering (Augedal et al., 2013; Hansen et al., 2013). In people who had PTSD in addition to the nightmare disorder, these post-traumatic stress symptoms also decreased
(Casement & Swanson, 2012; Krakow et al., 2001). Kunze and colleagues (2017) compared IR – as a standalone treatment – and imaginal exposure to a waiting list control condition in a group of 104 patients suffering from nightmare disorder. IR and imaginal exposure each consisted of three weekly sessions.

The frequency of the nightmares and the associated anxiety decreased significantly more in both active conditions than in the waiting list control condition. No difference was found between IR and imaginal exposure – they were equally effective. In summary, imagery rehearsal therapy is effective for the treatment of nightmare disorder. However, this treatment consists of several components, including cognitive restructuring. As far as we know, there is only one study that examined IR as a standalone treatment, and this was compared to another proven effective intervention (imaginal exposure). This study provides a first indication that IR, although not superior, can also be effective for nightmares.

Psychosis
Approximately three-quarters of psychotic patients report having negative mental images associated with their psychotic symptoms (Morrison et al., 2002). These images are thematically related to autobiographical memories (Schulze et al., 2013). Ison and colleagues (2014) describe a patient who saw images of a dead man with a bullet hole in his forehead. The autobiographical memories associated with these images were a suicide of her uncle when she was fourteen years old, after which she dreamed of his face under water, and an experience later in her life witnessing dead people in a burnt-down house. In people with auditory hallucinations, the images are related to the origin of the voice, or to what the voice is saying. Ison and colleagues (2014) also describe a 45-year-old woman who heard voices on the street calling her a child abuser. She described a mental image in which she saw how she was abused as a child. This image turned out to be related to memories of the sexual abuse by her brother when she was eleven years old.

Imagery rescripting for psychosis
So far, there is very limited evidence that IR could be effective in the treatment of psychotic disorders, as little research has been done on the subject.
There are two published case studies on the effectiveness of IR in delusions. IR was part of a more comprehensive treatment package including cognitive restructuring (Morrison, 2004; Serruya & Grant, 2009). Furthermore, two studies investigated the effect of IR in patients with auditory hallucinations. A single session of IR reduced the stressfulness of the intrusive images of memories associated with their auditory hallucinations for three out of four patients (Ison et al., 2014). Paulik and colleagues (2019) demonstrated that eight sessions of IR in 12 patients with auditory hallucinations drastically reduced both PTSD symptoms and auditory hallucinations. In summary, intrusive images seem to be common in people experiencing psychotic symptoms. Up to now, there has been only limited research into the possible effectiveness of IR for these difficulties. The available evidence does suggest that IR could have positive effects. However, far more research is needed.

Body dysmorphic disorder
Patients with a body dysmorphic disorder (BDD) are concerned about perceived bodily imperfections, such as wrinkles or the size or shape of the nose or eyes (APA, 2013). Although these imperfections are usually not visible to others, patients may spend hours a day checking their appearance, applying covering makeup or clothing, or regularly asking for reassurance. An increased self-centred attention, in which patients see themselves as an ‘aesthetic object’, seems to be a central feature of the disorder (Veale, 2004; Wilson et al., 2016). Cognitive-behavioural therapeutic models of the disorder emphasize the role of mental images as a sustaining factor (Veale, 2004; Veale & Neziroglu, 2010).

Patients with this disorder are more likely to have negative images of their appearance than subjects who do not. These images are often observed from the third-person perspective, where patients see themselves through the eyes of someone else (see Veale, 2004). Moreover, these images are more vivid, detailed and distorted, and are more often accompanied by physical sensations compared to those of a healthy control group (Osman et al., 2004). The content of the images is often related to memories of unpleasant events in childhood or adolescence. The most common memories concern bullying experiences or the physical changes that occurred during puberty (Buhlmann et al., 2007, 2011; Osman et al., 2004), or experiences of sexual and emotional abuse in the past (Neziroglu et al., 2006).

Imagery rescripting for body dysmorphic disorder

Given the central role that imagery seems to play in BDD and the strong association of the images with autobiographical memories, IR could be an effective intervention in this disorder. Although research into IR and BDD is limited, two studies with six patients each showed that one or two sessions of IR combined with cognitive restructuring led to a significant reduction in difficulties in most patients (Ritter & Stangier, 2016; Wilson et al., 2016), which sustained for (more than) half a year after the intervention. In summary, mental images seem to play a central role in BDD. These images appear to be strongly related to memories of meaningful experiences from the past. Research into the effectiveness of IR in BDD is very limited, but initial results seem favourable.

Personality disorders
Unlike PTSD, personality disorders are not particularly characterized by intrusive memories of specific traumatic events. People suffering from a personality disorder develop schemas in their youth that are based on meaningful, but not necessarily traumatic, youth experiences (Arntz 2011).

Imagery rescripting for personality disorders
IR can be used to process memories of meaningful childhood experiences. This application of IR is thought to be an effective way to treat personality disorders (Arntz & Van Genderen, 2009; Lobbestael et al., 2010; Young et al., 2003). However, due to the nature of the pathology, the treatment of personality disorders almost always consists of an extensive package of various methods and techniques. This makes it very difficult to investigate the isolated effect of IR. For example, IR is an important component of schema therapy, but it is certainly not the only one. Over the past fifteen years, schema therapy has been found to be highly effective for the treatment of personality disorders (e.g. Bamelis et al., 2014; Farrell et al., 2009; Giesen-Bloo et al., 2006; Nadort et al., 2009; Nordahl & Nysaeter, 2005; Van Asselt et al., 2008). It is likely that IR, as part of that treatment, has contributed to the effect. However, it is unclear whether this assumption is correct and to what extent IR was a primary component of the effectiveness of the therapy.

Other areas of application

IR is used in various other psychological problems. For example, IR was applied to a patient diagnosed with terminal cancer. The rescripting of childhood memories led to a reduction in anxiety and mood symptoms (Whitaker et al., 2010). Patients with agoraphobia appear to have images of agoraphobic situations that are associated with memories of unpleasant events in the past. Based on these findings, it is suggested that IR could be a possible part of the treatment of agoraphobia (Day et al., 2004). Furthermore, Reiss and colleagues (2018) found that cognitive behavioural therapy in combination with IR was an effective way to reduce subjective anxiety in students with test anxiety. The above studies illustrate the increasing interest in IR among people with various fear-related problems.

Summary

Most studies on the effectiveness of IR have been done in the field of PTSD and social anxiety disorder, and IR appears to be an effective treatment for these difficulties. Preliminary evidence suggests that IR could also be a good treatment strategy for depression. Moreover, the comorbid depressive symptoms were also found to decrease significantly when the intervention was applied to disorders other than depression (Morina et al. 2017). IR appears to be a promising, possibly complementary intervention in the treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder, nightmares, psychoses, eating disorders, specific phobias and personality disorders. However, it is important to bear in mind that research into the effectiveness of IR in these disorders has to date been limited and IR has not been compared to any other effective technique. Hence, it is not yet possible to give a definite answer about the effectiveness of IR in these disorders.

The working mechanism

Although there are now many studies that have demonstrated the effectiveness of IR in, for example, PTSD, there is still relatively little knowledge on the working mechanism of the intervention i.e. how the intervention produces its effect. The most common hypotheses explain the effect of IR either by a changed meaning of the representation of the meaningful autobiographical memory (Arntz, 2012) or through the creation of an alternative memory that competes with the original memory (Brewin, 2006, 2010; Stopa, 2010; Stopa & Jenkins, 2007). These two hypotheses are discussed in more detail below.

Hypothesis one: Imagery rescripting changes the meaning of the representation of autobiographical memory (Arntz, 2012)
This theory is based on the assumption that the mental representation of an event that is meaningful to the person, termed highly simplified as the ‘original memory’, enters a ‘labile state’ when retrieved in working memory. This labile state makes it possible to add information (e.g. through IR) and thus to change the intrinsic meaning of this representation (Arntz, 2012; Nader 2003). IR yields a stronger impact than a more verbal way of processing information (see e.g. Blackwell, 2018; Holmes & Mathews, 2010), and could change core beliefs at both cognitive as well as non-verbal/emotional levels (Arntz, 2011, 2015). In this way, IR can strengthen a sense of mastery (i.e. that something can be done about it), as opposed to the feelings of powerlessness and helplessness that were linked to the original memory (Kunze et al., 2019). By changing this meaning, the memory is stored in long-term memory with a different, less negative valence, which in theory should lead to fewer negative cognitions, feelings, and intrusions regarding the memory. Various experiments partly support this hypothesis (e.g. Dibbets et al., 2012, 2018; Hagenaars, 2012; Hagenaars & Arntz, 2012; Rijkeboer et al., 2019). However, many questions remain unanswered, so more research is needed.

Hypothesis two
: Imagery rescripting creates an alternative memory that competes with the original memory
This hypothesis states that in every situation, different relevant autobiographical memories compete for retrieval. In mental disorders, such as anxiety disorders, particularly negative, anxiety-confirming memories win the competition and are recalled in mind over memories that do not fit in that well to the experience of anxiety. Brewin (2006) argued that psychological treatments do not simply change negative memory representations. Rather, new, alternative representations are created that compete with the original, negative representations in terms of accessibility. From this point of view, the new representations need not so much be more realistic, but only need to be more easily accessible when patients are confronted with anxiety-inducing triggers (Brewin et al., 2010). According to this hypothesis, IR does not change the mental representation of a traumatic event, but instead a new, alternative representation is formed, which – hopefully – is often easier to retrieve than the negative variant (see also Arntz et al., 2013).

In conclusion, so far there seems to be little insight into the working mechanism of IR. The two most common hypotheses suggest a changing meaning of the original memory (Arntz 2012) or making an alternative mental representation more accessible (Brewin et al., 2010). But there may be other explanations for how IR works (see Arntz, 2012).
 
Dutch psychologist Herman Veerbeek developed the 'EMDR Directed Anger Protocol' which enables people to express anger against the perpetrator in the imagination. Anything the body feels it needs to do, it can. There are no limits. While its effectivity hasn't been researched as far as I know, clients have noted positive results (more peace of mind, less tension, and less anger). In a Dutch documentary, two clients are treated by Veerbeek using this protocol.

If I remember correctly, one of the two clients in the documentary is a war veteran who was mistreated by a general (the general gave him orders that put his life in danger and told him he's not allowed to talk back). He also experienced abuse by his mother. His traumatic experiences and the anger that had built over the years resulted in aggressive behavior. In the documentary, while working through the anger with the protocol, he breaks at some point and says how horrible he himself was (he was physically abusive in relationships). I think that was an important realization. Perhaps being able to express the anger he had toward others enabled him to truly look at himself.

Here's a video in English where the protocol is used (the client is an actor): Link

According to Veerbeek, after certain trauma, there's on the one hand fear and powerlessness, and on the other anger, grudge or resentment, revenge or vengeance. When there's more fear, powerlessness and anxiety, the regular EMDR protocol is used to process those. Once those are processed and anger comes in, the EMDR Directed Anger Protocol is used. If there's a mix of emotions, he asks the client to draw two buckets, one with anxiety and one with anger, and asks the client how full each feels to determine which protocol to use.

The difference between the EMDR Directed Anger Protocol and confronting the perpetrator through Imagery/Imaginal Rescripting (IR) is that with EMDR, only the client confronts the perpetrator, and it doesn't necessarily take place in the memory of a traumatic event. They can imagine the perpetrator anywhere and then do to them what the body 'needs'.

In IR (where you go back to the memory of a traumatic event - before the trauma occurs), the therapist can step in, the client's past self can step in, or the best version of their current self (also called "healthy adult"') steps in to protect their past self. Or all three. With IR, there is also the opportunity to fulfill the child's needs in that moment, for example the client might say his past self needs to be held and be comforted.

The EMDR Directed Anger Protocol is used in the Netherlands, and he's given workshops in Scotland, Germany and Australia. But it should be noted its effectivity hasn't been studied.

As a sidenote, when it comes to trauma, I've noticed that there are many layers to it when it comes to emotions and thoughts, sometimes intermixed. It can get pretty complex. With some clients I noticed there was first fear, then sadness (physical and emotional pain), then anger, and then grief.
 
And I daresay that for most intense events in our lives, this is effective enough to be processed during our sleep, however, more severe traumatic experiences may require the same method that REM sleep uses, only conscious.
That possibility is described here:

Sleep and PTSD​


There is a striking parallel between these physiological processes and the psychological theories implemented by therapists in the treatment of PTSD. For example, the therapist knows that progress cannot be made with the PTSD patient until she is able to discuss the traumatic event without replaying the episodic memory with its sensory and affective intensity. Only when these images are no longer intrusive can she integrate the event into her life, come to understand it, discover what it means for her, and thereby come to terms with it. Thus, a therapist strives, over time, to help a patient carry out these processes in the office—exposure therapy, for example may so overstimulate the episodic memory as to leave it refractory, allowing the therapist to help the patient integrate the experience cortically without interference from hippocampal intrusions. But this equally well describes the physiological processes of REM sleep—integration of the extracted and abstracted core of episodic memories into cortical, semantic memory networks, unimpeded by intrusive, hippocampal replay of the episodic memories. In both models, it is this integration that subsequently leads to the reduction in symptoms. What we now propose is that sleep,... and particularly REM sleep, have evolved to provide a privileged time during which the brain/mind is optimally tuned for exactly this type of memory transfer and integration.

When traumatic episodic memories are repetitively replayed in sleep, it is an indication that this system has broken down. Outside of PTSD, episodic memories are almost never replayed veridically in dreams (Stickgold et al., 2001). Although dreams contain “day residue,” this is usually in the form of factoids, not contextually accurate images or stories. Day residue enters our dreams as a character or phrase from the day, as an emotion or similar situation. But it does not appear as a replay of an actual event. For such a replay to occur would require the breakdown of the normal blockade of hippocampal outflow to the cortex, which, we propose, prevents the normal integration and subsequent weakening of the episodic memory. It is this sequence of events that we believe leads to PTSD.

The Physiology of REM Sleep and PTSD​


What could cause this disruption of sleep physiology? The sleep of PTSD sufferers is known to be more fragmented than normal and they appear to retain an inappropriate level of vigilance even while asleep (Mellman, Kumar, Kulick-Bell, & Nolan, 1995; Mellman, 1997). The amount of REM sleep may be reduced as well (Lavie, Hefez, Halperin, & Enoch, 1979; Hefez, Metz, & Lavie, 1987; Glaubman, Mikulincer, Porat, Wasserman, & Birger, 1990; but see Ross et al., 1994; Mellman, Kulick-Bell, Ashlock, & Nolan, 1995). Both of these phenomena could be explained by increased release of adrenal adrenaline or brainstem norepinephrine (NE) sufficient to produce a hyperaroused and hypervigilant condition with disrupted sleep. But NE also regulates the REM–non-REM cycle. During REM sleep, activity in the locus coeruleus and dorsal Raphe nucleus, brainstem structures which control levels of NE and serotonin in the brain, normally ceases (Hobson, McCarley, & Wyzinski, 1975). Failure to shut down these systems would lead to a dissociated neuromodulatory state and incomplete entry into REM sleep. Under such circumstances, we would expect associative processes to shift toward stronger associations and away from the weak associates normally activated during REM. They could also lead to a breach of the blockade of information flow from the hippocampus to the cortex, permitting the replay of traumatic memories.

Thus, the appearance of PTSD may result at least in part from the inability of the brain to inhibit norepinephrine or serotonin release during REM sleep. There is considerable evidence for elevated norepinephrine levels in PTSD patients both during wake (Bremner, Southwick, & Charney, 1999) and sleep (Mellman, Kumar, et al., 1995). Such an increase during sleep would block the activation of weak associations in the cortex (Stickgold et al., 1999), necessary for the integration of atypical (e.g., traumatic) memories into normative association networks, and would also disinhibit the blockade of hippocampal outflow (Buzsáki, 1996), leading to recurring reenactments of traumatic memories. With integration blocked, there would be no feedback to the hippocampus, and hence the hippocampus would fail to be sent instructions to weaken the episodic memory of the traumas and its associated negative affect. The consequence of this chain of events would be the self-sustaining condition of PTSD.

Brain imaging studies provide additional support for the concept that PTSD and REM sleep share functional pathways in the brain. Brain imaging studies have shown alterations in the activity of the hippocampus, amygdala, anterior cingulate, and possibly orbital frontal cortex, and visual cortex when PTSD patients are provoked with script driven imagery (Rauch et al., 1996; Shin et al., 1997). Yet these are precisely the brain regions that imaging studies suggest are activated during REM sleep (Hobson et al., 1998). Thus, the specific brain regions affected by restimulation of traumatic imagery in PTSD are the same ones activated during REM sleep. These findings support both the concept that traumatic memories are reprocessed during REM sleep and the hypothesis that PTSD may alter the normal functioning of the brain during REM sleep.

The Mechanism of Action of EMDR​

How might EMDR contribute to recovery from PTSD? If the particular physiological state encountered during REM sleep is supportive of the memory integration necessary... for recovery, then it is not unreasonable to conclude that interventions which shift the brain toward this state likewise would be beneficial. The question then is how EMDR might produce such a shift. Repeated saccadic eye movements could arguably “push-start” brainstem REM-induction mechanisms (Nelson, McCarley, & Hobson, 1983) through the reciprocal pathways that normally lead to the generation of rapid eye movements during REM sleep. Indeed, human brain imaging studies (Hong, Gillin, Dow, Wu, & Buchsbaum, 1995) indicate that eye movements in both REM sleep and wakefulness activate similar cortical areas. But visual pursuit and bilateral auditory and tactile stimulation require a different mechanism.

What all these techniques have in common, including those that utilize saccadic eye movements, is a pattern of alternating, bilateral stimulation that forces the subject to constantly shift her attention across the midline. It is this orienting response (Sokolov, 1990) that we propose induces a REM-like state, facilitating cortical integration of traumatic memories
.

The reorienting of attention can be triggered either intentionally, as when you choose to look at an object, or automatically, when a sudden movement “catches” your attention. This reorienting of attention requires first a release of focus from one location in visual space, then its shift to a new location, and finally its refocusing on this new location (Posner, 1980).

In extreme cases, for example a sudden loud noise, the automatic shift of attention is accompanied by an autonomic startle reaction (Koch, 1999). This startle response is biphasic, with an initial cholinergic activation that slows the heart (Fendt & Koch, 1999) and may also cause an automatic release of attention from its current focus (Davidson, Cutrell, & Marrocco, 1999). A fraction of a second later, release of adrenaline and norepinephrine causes the more familiar increase in heart rate, sweating, and muscle tensing associated with the adrenergic fight-and-flight response. But the brainstem surge of norepinephrine release also serves to shift and refocus the just-released attention to the location of the startling sound (Clark, Geffen, & Geffen, 1987).

What is most striking, aside from the momentary cholinergic activation reminiscent of REM sleep, is that the brainstem initiates a burst of “pontogeniculooccipital” (PGO) waves (Callaway, Lydic, Baghdoyan, & Hobson, 1987) in concert with the startle response (Bowker & Morrison, 1976). The only other condition known to generate PGO waves is REM sleep itself (Brooks & Bizzi, 1963). In fact, the neuronal circuits in the pontine brainstem that initiate PGO waves appears to control the REM–non-REM cycle as well. Local injection of acetylcholine into this PGO-generator brain region in the resting cat induces immediate and long-lasting REM sleep (Baghdoyan, Lydic, Callaway, & Hobson, 1989). Thus, inducing a startle response leads to activation of brainstem circuits that can also initiate REM sleep.

Cognitive tests have suggested that distinct brain systems are responsible for the release, shift, and then refocusing of attention of the orienting response (Posner & Dehaene, 1994). Among regions postulated to be involved are the anterior cingulate, discussed above in relation to both sleep and PTSD, and the superior colliculus, which controls eye movements, and which is activated by the PGO waves of REM sleep (Cespuglio, Laurent, & Calvo, 1976; Nelson et al., 1983). In addition, evidence suggests that cholinergic increases or noradrenergic decreases facilitate the release of attention prior to the shift (Clark et al., 1987; Davidson et al., 1999). Thus it seems reasonable to suggest that having a subject repetitively reorient her attention from one location to another could produce shifts in regional brain activation and neuromodulation similar to those produced during REM sleep.

In support of this hypothesis, Levin, Lazrove, and van der Kolk (1999) saw activation of the anterior cingulate and left frontal cortex following EMDR treatment, and Wilson, Silver, Covi, and Foster (1996) found decreased galvanic skin responses, suggestive of reduced adrenergic drive, with EMDR. It is precisely such shifts that we propose permit the changes in memory processing by which EMDR facilitates treatment of and demonstrably speeds recovery from PTSD.

Summary of the Model​


In summary, our model proposes that the constant reorienting of attention demanded... by the alternating, bilateral visual, auditory, or tactile stimuli of EMDR automatically activates brain mechanisms which facilitate this reorienting. Activation of these systems simultaneously shifts the brain into a memory processing mode similar to that of REM sleep. This REM-like state permits the integration of traumatic memories into associative cortical networks without interference from hippocampally mediated episodic recall. From a psychological perspective, the patient becomes able to see the significance and meaning of the event in terms of their overall life, and thereby to “come to terms” with the traumatic event. Once successfully integrated, corticohippocampal circuits induce the weakening of the traumatic episodic memory and its associated affect.

But EMDR can work even better than REM sleep for two specific reasons. First, unlike REM sleep, when frontal lobe activity is largely inhibited (Hobson et al., 1998), during EMDR treatment the patient can choose the material to hold in mind at the start of the bilateral stimulation, and thereby bias the information that will be processed. Although the selection of associations is largely automatic and unintentional, holding a specific image in mind at the start of the stimulation assures that the associations, however weak and tangential, will most likely be related to the original image. Second, through careful management by the therapist, levels of anxiety and fear during EMDR treatment largely can be maintained at low levels, enhancing the ability of the bilateral stimulation to produce the desired physiological and neurochemical shifts in the brain without interference from increased NE levels. In this manner, our model suggests that EMDR specifically reverses the breakdown of normal memory processing that initially leads to the development of PTSD. But, of course, this must be only a portion of the story.

In concluding, we wish to make clear that we are not suggesting that the breakdown in this memory processing pathway is either absolutely necessary or sufficient to produce and sustain PTSD, but rather that it plays an important role in the disease process, and, more importantly, that restoring the normal activity of this pathway can aid in recovery from PTSD. For example, breakdowns at other points in the process, such as the formation of the contextual pointers that encode the episodic memory, might explain the associated phenomenon of recall of traumatic memories through only a small number of sensory modalities (van der Kolk, 1999).

Nor are we the only ones to propose neurobiological or psychological models for the action of EMDR. Most obviously, this model bears a strong resemblance to that originally put forward by Shapiro (1989a, 1989b, 1995). She proposed a model of “accelerated information processing” (Shapiro, 1995, p. 28) that involved the formation of connections “to appropriate associations” (p. 29), and went on to suggest that the breakdown of this normal process might result from excessively high levels of norepinephrine and that the eye movements of EMDR might activate REM sleep mechanisms. Although our current model was developed independently, it may best be seen as narrowing the focus and deepening the physiological and cognitive neuroscience underpinnings of Shapiro’s model. Alternative, more psychologically defined models for a role of the orienting response in EMDR have been proposed by Lipke (1995, 2000) and by Armstrong and Vaughan (1996), and both Bergmann (1998) and Servan-Schreiber (2000) have recently proposed a model based on enhanced interhemispheric communication and synchronous neuronal firing patterns. Like ours, these models focus on the ability of EMDR to produce an altered mind-brain state in which effective processing of the traumatic memories can occur.

These theories are testable. Servan-Schreiber has begun tests of his model, and we are now looking at whether EMDR produces an enhancement of weak associations using the same cognitive test with which we showed this effect after REM sleep awakenings (Stickgold et al., 1999). Other studies can be imagined. We would predict, for example, that formal analysis of the dreams of PTSD patients would show increased overall incorporation of episodic memories into their dreams. We would similarly expect an alteration in the normal sleep onset replay of recent experiences (Stickgold, Malia, Maguire, Roddenberry, & O’Connor, 2000). Whether the results of such studies support or oppose these theories, they will move us forward in our understanding of the brain basis of EMDR.
In short, as summarized by AI:

Sleep, especially REM sleep, helps process memories by reducing their emotional intensity, but in PTSD, this process breaks down due to disrupted sleep and elevated stress hormones like norepinephrine, leading to intrusive traumatic memories replayed in dreams. EMDR therapy, using eye movements or bilateral stimulation, mimics REM sleep to help integrate these memories, making them less distressing. Research shows that both REM sleep and PTSD involve similar brain areas, like the hippocampus and amygdala, and that EMDR may restore normal memory processing by creating a REM-like state, allowing patients to better cope with trauma. But additional research is needed.
 
That possibility is described here:
That's super interesting, it also makes me think about meditation exercises in which people "zone out", whenever it has happened to me, fall asleep during meditation exercises, it feels like sleep, but somehow it isn't . It might be close to EMDR perhaps, or a similar physiological process might take place, albeit closer to sleep than conscious therapy, but not quite all the way there.
 

Trending content

Back
Top Bottom