The Polyvagal Theory - Stephen W. Porges

Digging through more info to see if there's a relationship between RH and a ventral vagal state. I understand from previous studies that fear and anger can narrow focus - reduce the available information that can be considered from the perceptive field and fear and anger could most likely be sympathetically driven from the perspective of PVT.

Deb Dana says that her clients report an expansion in their peripheral field of view when she helps them access a ventral vagal state. McGilchrist says that left hemisphere concentrates on particular focal details and right hemisphere has a more wholistic perception. Not sure yet, but that might mean that RH has a relationship to a ventral vagal state. Looked up some peripheral vision exercises as I thought that along the lines of Porges Safe and Sound Protocol that works on the acoustic pathways to vagal toning, there might be some visual pathways given that Dana uses nature films without the sound to similar effect. So, does being in nature prompt more peripheral vision attention? This article suggests that peripheral vision exercises might stimulate the release of acetylcholine (controls the heart rate) - I haven't yet found anything else to confirm that.

Porges states that an emergent property of a ventral vagal state is greater prosody and intonation of voice. McGilchrist says (in this video that's worthwhile watching for a lot of reasons given his fascinating work - How our brains turned fools woke) that in RH damaged people where the LH is predominant, they cannot take into account differences in intonation and adjust contextual perception to take that information into account. He gives the example of saying the word 'yes' in different moods, eg. how a yes sounds if you really don't want to do something, how a resigned yes sounds, how an exuberant yes sounds, how a victorious yes sounds, etc. The RH can take in the additional contextual information, but the LH will only hear the word yes without the additional contextual information.
 
Deb Dana says that her clients report an expansion in their peripheral field of view when she helps them access a ventral vagal state.
That’s quite interesting & I’m not sure what the correlation of vagal nerve stimulation is to peripheral vision, purely on a mechanical level (more on this below). The optic nerve is a sensory nerve, relaying information to the visual cortex, so no autonomic nerves are present. However, there are both sympathetic & parasympathetic nerves to other parts of the eye, (which regulate pupil size, lens accommodation, ocular circulation & intraocular pressure), which in turn may have secondary effects to resultant peripheral vision & certainly depth perception.

From what we have read in McGilchrist’s book, it would appear that the R hemisphere (HS) is linked to the sympathetic fright/flight system, with noradrenaline as its neurotransmitter, while the L HS controls the parasympathetic system & uses dopamine. It almost seems like it should be the other way around. However, the left HS likes the known & the familiar which makes it feel safe (parasympathetic nervous system), while the right is in correspondence with the Other/Something-beyond-the-self i.e. the unknown. It kind of makes sense then, that the R HS regulates the sympathetic (fear of the unknown), while the L HS regulates the parasympathetic nervous system. Below is a slide from our workshop discussion on The Master and his Emissary, and a webpage that summarises the differences between the HSs very well (picture is from that site).

E8229036-9E05-4596-ADFD-112B4A824739.jpeg

438AC6CB-1422-41F3-9B5F-DE83A8F721D8.jpeg



With regards to purely a mechanical (& very much a L HS congruent) point of view of biology & physiology, I think there is still so much we don’t know about how the brain communicates with itself. There are now biophotons that have been discovered in the brain, suggesting optical communication rather than just neurotransmitters and physical processes. They are also proposing that messages transmitted along nerve fibres are bi/mutildirectional instead of unidirectional, with quantum entanglement & other quantum phenomena occurring at a cellular level. A thread on biophotons here and Pierre’s research into it, which is quite fascinating.

Perhaps the ability to really see (including a widened field of vision) has more to do with which hemisphere one tends to more habitually operate out of. As McGilchrist says, it’s not just a way of seeing, but a way of being in the world (a fundamental disposition to the world), something I personally feel may be tied in with FRV and ones’s Knowledge/Awareness/Being.
 
It almost seems like it should be the other way around.

Yeah it does! And I think there's a clue as to what may be going on. It seems as though that when he is referring to the autonomic nervous system, McGilchrist is speaking from the old paradigm of there just being the sympathetic and the parasympathetic systems operating in tension with each other. But the dorsal vagus is part of the parasympathetic system and it is not addressed in the old paradigm.

There's a paper here where Porges talks about what he refers to the vagal paradox where he addresses that. When he talks about how he learned about the vagal paradox, says that he'd written a paper about all these great things about the vagus nerve, and then was contacted by a neonatalist who had read his paper and said that they'd learned that the vagus nerve could kill you. Apparently preterm babies who are prone to bradycardia and apnea are in a state of dorsal vagal shutdown.

Now it makes sense that LH is parasympathetic because there are degrees to a dorsal vagal shut down that begin with dissociation in the mildest sense, and McGilchrists characteristics of LH, while they may be parasympathetic don't seem to fit very well with the characteristics of ventral vagal social engagement.

Still not so sure about RH being sympathetic though. It makes sense that if RH has a more wholistic view, it's more likely to be aware of potential threats in the environment, but there is a difference, I think, of being aware of threats in the environment and operating from a state of fight/flight. The listed characteristics of RH just don't seem to fit with fight/flight. If ventral vagal is social engagement, and dorsal and sympathetic describe various states and degrees of disconnection, then RH characteristics don't seem to align. However, when there is no threat, dorsal, sympathetic and vagal all have positive roles in maintaining homeostasis in the body and it's only when it's recruited during stress or threat that the sympathetic nervous system produces fight/flight so maybe I'm over thinking it.
 
Still not so sure about RH being sympathetic though. It makes sense that if RH has a more wholistic view, it's more likely to be aware of potential threats in the environment, but there is a difference, I think, of being aware of threats in the environment and operating from a state of fight/flight. The listed characteristics of RH just don't seem to fit with fight/flight. If ventral vagal is social engagement, and dorsal and sympathetic describe various states and degrees of disconnection, then RH characteristics don't seem to align. However, when there is no threat, dorsal, sympathetic and vagal all have positive roles in maintaining homeostasis in the body and it's only when it's recruited during stress or threat that the sympathetic nervous system produces fight/flight so maybe I'm over thinking it.

Interesting.

I agree that ventral vagal (which is parasympathetic) must have something of Right Hemisphere as it is a state that is relational, context-aware and emphatic, so it can't be completely of the Left hemisphere, IMO (of course, I just say this without being an expert on the subject).

I thought too about on the right hemisphere in terms of what McGilchrist mentions about how it is the Right Hemisphere that looks for threats/changes/novelty in the surrounding environment with peripheral vision, for example, while the Left hemisphere is more focused on details (this is more in relation to animals like birds, but still) And I thought that that is pretty similar to what some say is a ventral vagal state in which you can be calm but alert at the same time, meaning that you are aware of your environment and surroundings while still being in relaxed state and focused on your 'task' at hand, so to say.

I'm sure there's a lot still to learn and understand about how the brain works and that it may be even more complex. I guess it is important to remember that both hemispheres actually work together, or that the aim is for them to work together, just like with the nervous system states, what is good isn't to always be in just one state but to have flexibility among different states, and to be able to come back to ventral vagal, that's why the heart rate variability is a measure of health in general and nervous system regulation particularly.

Having said that, could it be that what is described as ventral vagal is actually a state in which there's input from both hemispheres in a more balanced way? I really don't know, but it's an interesting idea. :-)
 
@Arwenn wrote:
“Perhaps the ability to really see (including a widened field of vision) has more to do with which hemisphere one tends to more habitually operate out of. As McGilchrist says, it’s not just a way of seeing, but a way of being in the world (a fundamental disposition to the world), something I personally feel may be tied in with FRV and ones’s Knowledge/Awareness/Being.”

From my observations and intensive studies of “Biological programs” which are described as “Biological Laws” by certain “outlier groups, my response is.... this “fundamental disposition to the world” that you mentioned, IS obviously biological laterality, not a “habit”, but a biological lateral wiring, in the brain.

“Laterality” simply means every the sentient being, is either left handed or right handed.
A left spin, or right spin, happens in the first moments of conception, and the hemisphere wiring begins.

From the “Learning GNM site”:
“In the practical application of GNM it is of utmost importance to ascertain a person’s biological handedness, because the handedness determines whether the conflict impacts on the right or left side of the brain and whether a symptom (skin rash, muscle weakness, rheumatic pain, breast cancer) occurs on the right or left side of the body, taking into account the cross-over correlation from the brain to the organ (the brain-organ relation is always unequivocal).”


NOTE: The biological handedness is established at the moment of the first cell division after conception. This is why with identical twins one is biologically right-handed and one is left-handed. Many left-handed people were retrained in early childhood in order to fit into the right-handed world. The real ratio of right-handers and left-handers is approximately 60:40.
 
During the course of writing my thesis, in which I use Porges' Polyvagal Theory as a foundation in one of the chapters, I found out that there have been quite dishonest and ugly attempts to smear and take down Porges and his theory by a couple of scientists since around 2007. The main 'debunkers' and harrassers appear to be two scientists, Paul Grossman from University Hospital Basel (emeritus research director in psychosomatic medicine), and Edwin W. Taylor from University of Birmingham (emeritus prof. at school of biosciences). The more zealous of these two appears to be Grossman, who has used social media to spread his 'takedown' of PVT. As a sign of his unhealthy commitment, he's registered on Twitter X by the username (at)Paulyvagal. :scared:

I've been reading the central articles by Grossman (2007, 2023), and also the recent response by Porges (2023, see below), and even without expertice in the field (but having some expertice in spotting shenanigan tactics!), it is clear to me that what Grossman is doing is dishonest and without basis. Porges, in his response, makes it clear that Grossman uses good ol' strawman tactics, and with his use of social media and internet sites, he has managed to convince many people, at least outside of academia. As usual, the Wikipedia page on PVT is edited to suit the agenda of the 'dark side'. Amongst other things, there you can find the following gem:

There is consensus among experts that the assumptions of the polyvagal theory are untenable. PVT is popular among some clinical practitioners and patients, but it is not endorsed by current social neuroscience.

To get a summary of what's been going on you can start by reading Porges', more popularized, answer here:

Which is his response to the article: R.I.P. Polyvagal Theory:

Following this, Grossman recently started a thread on Researchgate called
'Why does the polyvagal conglomerate of conjectures continue to enjoy popularity despite its basic biological premises having been refuted?'.

Notice the part "having been refuted"! :pinocchio:

If you read the above text and especially his comments to his 'minions' below, you'll maybe get the sense, as I did, that his motivation for writing is coming from a place of jealousy, the 'sexual center', or even 4DSTS. This can also be witnessed even in his scientific articles in which he uses wordings like 'highly implaucible', 'category mistake', 'falsified', which is uncommon to see in scientific articles that usually are quite diplomatic and careful in their criticism.

If you want to get to the details , here are the central articles to read:

Grossman (and Taylor):
Fundamental challenges and likely refutations of the five basic premises of the polyvagal theory (2023)
Toward understanding respiratory sinus arrhythmia: relations to cardiac vagal tone, evolution and biobehavioral functions (2007)

Porge's response:
The vagal paradox: A polyvagal solution (2023) [the central points to refute Grossman and Taylor are towards the end of the article]

Now, what bugs me the most with this smearing campaign is that it's an, maybe intentional, attack against the central aspects of humanity and compassion. This is explained best by a couple of quotes from the new book by Porges and his son Seth that I'm currently reading called 'Our Polyvagal World: How Safety and Trauma Change Us' (highly recommended!):

The Polyvagal Theory also offers us a way of approaching the world that allows us to understand human nature on a profound, empathetic, and satisfying level. It provides an explanation for what our chronically unsafe, traumatic, and anxiety-filled world does to our health—and how we can reclaim our bodies and brains from the grips of a world that seems designed to scare and hurt us (p. XVII).

and

To live in a state of fear is to live a life in a body that is tuned away from health, growth, restoration, happiness, and sociality. Life is shorter and harder. This state is a slow-motion killing machine that eats away at our bodies and spirits, causing real damage to our organs while acting as a numbing or dissociative depressant that keeps us from enjoying life. It also changes the way we experience the world—and usually not for the better. When we feel unsafe, our very senses change. Things taste, look, smell, and sound different. Experiences we once loved lose their luster. Our ability to live, learn, and think critically evaporates in favor of an immediate need for survival. We become easy to manipulate and susceptible to following the direction of our most craven and cynical peers (p. XVIII).

and

If we were capable of seeing the world around us only in terms of threats, we’d be like reptiles that sit in isolation, waiting to eat or be eaten. [...] The more threatened our nervous system feels, the more primitive our response. As we get more defensive and fearful, the higher thinking that is unique to (and in many ways makes us) humans is bypassed in favor of reactionary gut instincts. When we feel unsafe, we effectively devolve back in time from freethinking modern humans to savage and scared vertebrates—and possibly even immobilized lizards (pp. 24, 36-37).

At leats to me, this smear campaign smells like 4DSTS/Lizzie inspired! :scared::curse:
 
I'm glad this criticism of PVT was brought up, Aragorn. I have come across it and had planned to write a post on it. For those who prefer to listen or watch, Porges also addresses the slur campaign against the theory here:


The interesting thing in regards to these weak criticisms leading with fallacies that I have observed after listening up to 6hrs a week of interviews with Porges for a number of months is that every now and then an interviewer would ask a question that might uncover something about characteropathies and Porges in a smooth, seemingly practiced and formulaic manner, redirects the conversation away from where the interviewer is leading and back towards either trauma responses or autism in a firm manner - even when the interview then reframes the question and asks it again. The feeling that I get is that he knows something, but he ain't going there or so it seems to me. It could be because he is of the opinion that all antisocial behaviour is the result of trauma, but that's not the sense I get from his redirection because there really seems to be a subtle shift in his demeanour that suggests that something else is going on.

A while back I expressed an interest in hearing an interview with both Porges and McGilchrist. For those not aware, McGilchrist does address the probable pathologising influence of a totally left brained view of the world and the impact that has on society, the world, arts and sciences etc. I contacted McGilchrist through his webpage and registered an interest in a joint interview between him and Porges. I got a response that said something along the lines of 'We'd really like that too, but an opportunity hasn't come up.' Whether an opportunity will ever come up is a wait and see.

Given the C's statements re PVT, I haven't given up on finding an answer yet.
 
Thanks Jones, that's interesting!

This was, and is, a good excercise to learn to see what's most relevant and of real importance in the debate. For instance, around here we pretty much agree that the Darwinian theory of evolution is false, or at least full of holes. So, when Porges and Grossman (with his posse) argue about the evolutionary/phylogenetic details in the PVT, it's from our perspective irrelevant. Heck, who knows, maybe Porges doesn't believe in Darwinism himself, but he has to play by the 'academic rules'. The game would be lost if you'd throw in the mix ideas of how evolution and Darwin's ideas are wrong. So, even if the debate over the evolutionary details is, in a sense, irrelevant for us with our 'Cassipaean cosmology', you can still see how Porges is being attacked with false strawman arguments.

One hilarious, and a bit scary, example of this is the Grossman camp arguing how reptiles do, indeed, have an active social life similar to mammals! This they use as a strawman: Porges claims that the PVT explains how there are connections between mammal behavior and bodily functions and social interactions and e.g. facial and phonatory cues (prosody) => reptilians have an active social life too, so the whole PVT is proven false!

Anyone who sees reptilian 'social life' as similar to what mammals are engaged in must have something dark inside of him. :cool2:
 
Now, what bugs me the most with this smearing campaign is that it's an, maybe intentional, attack against the central aspects of humanity and compassion. This is explained best by a couple of quotes from the new book by Porges and his son Seth that I'm currently reading called 'Our Polyvagal World: How Safety and Trauma Change Us' (highly recommended!):
Thank you for the recommendation, Aragorn. I am really interested in the polyvagal theory, and I started reading the book last night. I am only a newbie to the theory itself, but to me it makes a lot of sense. Also, I listened to Laura's second interview with Jake and Hunter again where they discuss imprinting and the barbaric circumcision of 8 days old Jewish baby boys who then go on to develop a hatred against women (and harbour a murderous rage against humanity,) so it's not surprising that they go after Stephen Porges. But that's just my take on it.

I read Porges's article on LinkedIn and although it is unsurprising I wanted to include this quote:
In Chapter 2 I am attempting to provide the community of curious scientists and therapists interested in Polyvagal Theory with an understanding of the weaknesses of the Grossman and Taylor arguments. As you read the chapter, you will share my surprise that they were ignorant of the specific theory that they were criticizing. From my perspective, their criticisms are not relevant to Polyvagal Theory." Prof. Stephen Porges
 
Porges has also put his response to the criticism on his PVT page:

Dr. Porges Responds to Polyvagal Theory Critiques​

Abridged by Polyvagal Institute with permission, August 2023​

CRITICISM:
In a 2023 review of the literature, Paul Grossman states that "there is broad consensus among experts that each basic physiological assumption of the polyvagal theory is untenable. Much of the existing evidence, upon which these consensuses are grounded, strongly indicates that the underlying polyvagal hypotheses have been falsified." Grossman, Paul (2023). Fundamental challenges and likely refutations of the five basic premises of the polyvagal theory. Biological Psychology. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2023.108589.

RESPONSE FROM DR. PORGES:
Statements like this as ‘criticism’ are not very constructive in resolving any disagreement. First, what data substantiates his statement of consensus? Who are the experts with sufficient knowledge of the theory to make these judgments? Second, which specific assumptions is he referring to? Without a clear statement of the theory and the assumptions associated with the theory the ‘tenability’ of any aspect of the theory cannot be judged.
If we define ‘consensus’ acceptance of the theory within peer reviewed publications, then PVT would be viewed as having broad consensus among peers involved in the research relevant to PVT. This broad acceptance is substantiated by the approximately 20,000 peer reviewed articles in academic science.
Statements like this, often supported by research conducted by Edwin Taylor, and repeated by Paul Grossman, have frequently been used to dispute PVT. However, as documented in Porges (2023), Taylor’s papers have repeatedly misrepresented PVT; they have argued that their misrepresentations, and not any specific aspect of the theory, are evidence that PVT is untenable. In dealing with any criticism of PVT, it is first necessary to have the critic articulate the point of disagreement, which requires the critic to document how the point of contention is actually represented in PVT and why it is wrong.
If there were truly ‘broad consensus’ documenting that the underlying polyvagal hypotheses had been falsified and the foundational science upon which PVT rests was truly unsubstantiated, it would represent a major consequence to our understanding of many biologically informed sciences. If this falsification was substantiated it would have effectively falsified evolution and how evolution is mirrored in embryology. It would have falsified the Jacksonian principle of dissolution (a guiding principle in neurology), it would have falsified Taylor’s documentation of ventral migration of cardioinhibitory neurons in the brainstem, and it would have falsified Grossman’s own research documenting that the suppression of RSA is a vagal phenomenon. Finally, this quote, by not inviting dialog or alternative perspectives, makes such sweeping and biased claims that it’s hard not to question what the critic actually knows about PVT.
See: Sections 17 and 18 of the Vagal Paradox paper for more detail.











CRITICISM:
Neuhuber and Berthoud (2022) state that polyvagal theory's "basic phylogenetic and functional-anatomical tenets do not withstand closer scrutiny". They argue that polyvagal theory incorrectly portrays the role of the different vagal nuclei in mediating the freeze response. According to their analysis, the evidence "does not support a role of the 'dorsal vagal complex' in freezing as proposed by the PVT (Porges, 2001)" and the dorsal vagal complex "should not be linked to passive defensive behavior". Neuhuber, Winfried; Berthoud, Hans-Rudolf (2022-10-01). Functional anatomy of the vagus system: How does the polyvagal theory comply?. Biological Psychology. 174: 108425. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2022.108425. PMID 36100134. S2CID 252188181.

RESPONSE FROM DR. PORGES:
Scientists are entitled to their opinion, but an opinion does not falsify a theory. PVT emphasizes the role of the different vagal circuits in supporting a threat physiology. It uses supporting literature that proposes the species-specific responses of prey species who do freeze. See section 3, paragraph 8, as well as sections 11-13 of the Vagal Paradox paper.
But, more specifically, in the updated paper (Porges, 2023) the role of the dorsal vagal complex is elaborated to include widely accepted threat reactions including reduction of blood pressure that potentially could trigger fainting and the frequently reported experienced effect of threat on the gut. In discussing dorsal vagal defense reactions, PVT acknowledges that the neural pathways are still being explored in research and that the most reliable vagal effect mediated via the dorsal vagus may not be heart rate slowing, but reduction of contractility leading to a drop in blood pressure as well as profound effects on digestion resulting in bowel evacuation and gut pain. Personally, as research progresses, I am curious to learn more about how the dorsal vagus can shift its role from supporting homeostatic function by protecting the myocardium and the gut to a pathway that would disrupt homeostatic function with potential damaging consequences.





CRITICISM:
Regarding the proposed "ventral vagal complex," Neuhuber and Berthoud state that "the PVT, by construing a 'new ventral vagal complex' encompassing the entire branchiomotor column ascribed to the vagus much more than it actually can serve." They see it as "misleading to propose that brainstem branchiomotor ('source') nuclei 'communicate directly with the visceromotor portion of the nucleus ambiguus' (Porges, 2001)," and conclude that the relevant networks "should not be termed 'ventral vagal complex.' This terminology may insinuate that the vagus is a 'prime mover.' This is not the case [...]."

RESPONSE FROM DR. PORGES:
This is a good example of incorrect paraphrasing. PVT goes to great lengths to focus not on the vagus but on the brainstem area from which the vagus emerges. The vagus is a conduit in PVT, which is far from a ‘prime mover.’ Neuhuber and Berthoud are entitled to their opinion, but that does not substantiate their incorrect inference.
It is important to note that the central vagal complex as proposed in PVT is the product of a ventral migration of cardioinhibitory neurons from the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus to a brainstem area that regulates the striated muscles of the face and head. It is this ventral migration that provided the neuroanatomical substrate that enabled mammals to nurse and subsequently to engage in social behavior as a co-regulatory system. It is clear that this point is not understood in framing the above criticism, which does not appreciate the conceptualization of the social engagement system as the functional output of the interneuronal connections among the nuclei composing the ventral vagal complex. These points are elaborated in Porges (2023) in Sections 6-9.






CRITICISM:
Taylor, Wang & Leite (2022) regard it as "invalid to refer to this as a 'vagal system' or to postulate the existence of a 'smart vagus.'" Taylor, Edwin; Wang, Tobias; Leite, Cleo (2022-06-28). An overview of the phylogeny of cardiorespiratory control in vertebrates with some reflections on the 'Polyvagal Theory'. Biological Psychology. 172: 108382.

RESPONSE FROM DR. PORGES:
Again, this is an opinion and as long as the definitions are clear it should not be ‘invalid’ to use a term not in agreement with other opinions. Initially, as PVT emphasized the link between intentionality (overt behavior) and ventral vagal function, the ventral vagus was labeled the ‘smart’ vagus. This was later dropped (at least 20 years ago), since it implied an ‘executive’ function, which was not the intention. The term was meant to highlight the autonomic state that might optimize higher brain functions while the dorsal vagus was initially labeled the ‘vegetative’ vagus since it was involved in background homeostatic processes as well as being recruited in survival reactions. It would have been helpful to Taylor and his group in framing their criticisms, if they carefully read the primary papers describing the theory. In response to a changing scientific background, the theory has been refined and updated several times during the 30 years since its inception.





CRITICISM:
Grossman and Taylor (2007) argue that there is no evidence that the dorsal motor nucleus (DMN) is an evolutionarily more primitive center of the brainstem parasympathetic system than the nucleus ambiguus (NA), and review evidence to the contrary. Grossman, Paul; Taylor, Edwin W. (2007-02-01). Toward understanding respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Relations to cardiac vagal tone, evolution and biobehavioral functions. Biological Psychology. 74 (2): 263–285. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.11.014.

RESPONSE FROM DR. PORGES:
This is an uninformed criticism of PVT. The following is taken from section 18 of the recently published Vagal Paradox paper:

Taylor and colleagues have also questioned the assumption that the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus is an evolutionarily older structure than the ventral vagus. It has been reliably documented that prior to mammals the prominent cardioinhibitory vagal neurons in vertebrates originated in the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus. Thus, it is indisputable that estimating an evolutionary timeline through phylogeny, cardioinhibitory neurons originated first in the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus and then, consistent with Taylor’s own work [36], migrated ventrally. In the earliest (now extinct) mammals this ventral migration was sufficiently complete to embed cardioinhibitory functions with activities of branchiomotor neurons (i.e., special visceral efferent pathways) that regulate the striated muscles of the face and head promoting ingestion (e.g., nursing) and social communication via facial expression and vocalizations.





CRITICISM:
A more recent paper by Monteiro et al. (2018) finding myelinated vagus nerve fibers of lungfish leading from the nucleus ambiguus to the heart also indicates that polyvagal theory’s hypothesis that the nucleus ambiguus is unique to mammals is incorrect. They state that "the mechanisms [Porges] identifies as solely mammalian are undeniably present in the lungfish that sits at the evolutionary base of the air-breathing vertebrates." Monteiro, Diana (2018). Cardiorespiratory interactions previously identified as mammalian are present in the primitive lungfish. Science Advances. 4 (2): eaaq0800. Bibcode:2018

RESPONSE FROM DR. PORGES:
The following is taken from section 9 and section 18 of the recently published Vagal Paradox paper:

Describing respiratory-heart rate patterns in other vertebrates does not mean that the neural mechanisms are identical to those observed in mammals. In fact, in vertebrate species other than mammals, except for the report of a myelinated cardioinhibitory pathway emerging from the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus in the lungfish [49], all reports document that heart rate-respiratory interactions were mediated via unmyelinated vagal cardioinhibitory pathways originating in the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus. The identification of the myelinated fibers in the lungfish have been misused to infer a ‘fatal flaw’ in PVT. However, the identification of myelinated vagal fibers in lungfish is unrelated to PVT and reflects a misunderstanding of PVT. The lungfish appears to be a phylogenetic outlier, having vertebrate ancestry that did not have myelinated cardioinhibitory dorsal vagal fibers nor has this feature been reliably transmitted to the groups of vertebrates that subsequently evolved (i.e., amphibia, reptiles, mammals).




CRITICISM:
Grossman (2023) concurs, stating that "the polyvagal notion that the ventral vagal area is unique to mammals is opposed by years of evidence" and that the "findings, as a whole, firmly and consistently contradict the polyvagal hypotheses that propose the [dorsal vagal motor nucleus] as the “source nucleus” of unmyelinated pathways and the [nucleus ambiguus] as the “source nucleus” of myelinated pathways in mammals." Grossman, Paul (2023). Fundamental challenges and likely refutations of the five basic premises of the polyvagal theory. Biological Psychology. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2023.108589.

RESPONSE FROM DR. PORGES:
This is another uninformed statement. PVT emphasizes that the source nucleus of myelinated vagal pathways conveying a respiratory rhythm in mammals is the ventral vagus. This does NOT occur in reptiles. This point is consistent with the research literature for several decades.
See Section 18 of The Vagal Paradox for more detail.




CRITICISM:
Results reviewed by Taylor, Leite and Skovgaard (2010) "refute the proposition that centrally controlled cardiorespiratory coupling is restricted to mammals, as propounded by the polyvagal theory of Porges."
Taylor; Leite; Skovgaard (2010). Autonomic control of cardiorespiratory interactions in fish, amphibians and reptiles.

In Taylor, Wang & Leite's 2022 review, the evidence for the presence of cardio-respiratory interactions similar to respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) and their potential purpose in blood oxygenation in many vertebrate species (both air- and water-breathing) leads them to conclude that RSA may be a relic of older cardio-respiratory systems, contrary to polyvagal assumptions. Taylor, Edwin; Wang, Tobias; Leite, Cleo (2022-06-28). An overview of the phylogeny of cardiorespiratory control in vertebrates with some reflections on the 'Polyvagal Theory.' Biological Psychology. 172: 108382.

RESPONSE FROM DR. PORGES:
In this section Taylor and his research team repeatedly confuse mammalian RSA, which is dependent on myelinated vagal fibers originating in the ventral vagus (nucleus ambiguus) that have an inherent respiratory rhythm, with non-mammalian cardio-respiratory coupling that involves vagal fibers from the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus. This criticism reflects a feature of Taylor's repeated strawman arguments. PVT does NOT make this proposition.





CRITICISM:
The dichotomy between asocial reptiles and social mammals subscribed to by polyvagal theory has been contested. Doody, Burghardt & Dinets (2023) consider several ways of assessing and classifying animal sociality and state that "Porges’ dichotomy is incorrect. While many mammals (particularly humans) may show more complex social behavior than reptiles, there is considerable overlap in social tendencies between the two groups. The labels ‘social’ and ‘asocial’ are too crude to have utility in a comparative framework of social behavior and should not be used to describe taxa. Listing examples of social behavior in reptiles and other non-mammal vertebrates, they observe that "PT appears to rest upon 20th century folk interpretation of vertebrate evolutionary biology rather than on current scientific understanding of it."
Doody; Burghardt; Dinets (2023). The evolution of sociality and the polyvagal theory. Biological Psychology.

RESPONSE FROM DR. PORGES:
The following reply is excerpted from Section 18 of the newly published Vagal Paradox paper:

These criticisms are irrelevant to PVT, which is mammal centric. Sociality through a polyvagal lens focuses on the transformative qualities of social behavior expressed in mammals and not observed in reptiles such as nursing during mother-infant interactions, vocal intonations, facial expressions, and other co-regulatory behaviors that have profound impact on calming autonomic state to optimize homeostatic functions. This criticism is not only applying the theory to questions in another discipline, but to a question (i.e., social behavior in reptiles) that has been explicitly stated to be outside the scope of the theory.




CRITICISM:
Polyvagal theory proposes a relationship between RSA responses and forms of psychopathology, but a meta-analysis finds the empirical evidence to be inconclusive. Beauchaine, Theodore P.; Bell, Ziv; Knapton, Erin; McDonough‐Caplan, Heather; Shader, Tiffany; Zisner, Aimee (2019). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia reactivity across empirically based structural dimensions of psychopathology: A meta-analysis. Psychophysiology. 56 (5): e13329.

RESPONSE FROM DR. PORGES:
The PVT does not make that proposition, since it does not adhere to psychophysiological parallelism (see Porges, S. W., 2022, Polyvagal Theory: A Science of Safety. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 16, 27.) in which a physiological marker would be a defining feature of mental health diagnosis. However, research informed by PVT would test hypotheses that might investigate such a relationship. A PVT informed hypothesis would focus on compromised vagal regulation being more frequently observed in individuals with specific clinical disorders such as depression or anxiety. Thus, there might be bias within these clinical diagnostic groups to be in autonomic states of defense. In general this is the case, and I have published several papers in this general area. A simple Google Scholar search will identify hundreds of papers documenting association between indices of RSA and mental health features. This does not preclude that there may be a large overlap in the distributions between those without diagnoses and diagnosed participants. The relationship is far from causal and its relationship with PVT is linked to the autonomic characteristics mediating mental health symptoms and NOT mental health diagnoses being defined by autonomic features.





CRITICISM:
According to Grossman and Taylor, the existing research indicates that respiratory sinus arrhythmia is not a reliable marker of vagal tone, since it is subject to both respiratory variables and sympathetic (beta-adrenergic) influences in addition to vagal influences. Grossman, Paul; Taylor, Edwin W. (2007-02-01). Toward understanding respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Relations to cardiac vagal tone, evolution and biobehavioral functions. Biological Psychology. 74 (2): 263–285. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.

RESPONSE FROM DR. PORGES:
This conclusion is dependent on Grossman and Taylor using a poor metric to quantify RSA. The vulnerability of their metric has been substantiated in peer reviewed publications (see Lewis et al. 2011; Byrne & Porges, 1996). However, when an appropriate method is used, RSA maps into the cholinergic influence (see Lewis et al., 2011, Porges, 1986). Recent papers (e.g., Gourine et al., 2016) emphasize that the respiratory pattern in HR (RSA) is a reliable marker of ‘ventral vagal tone.’ Interestingly, this phenomenon was reported by Hering in 1910, although it took decades to refine the methodology to accurately monitor ventral vagal tone via RSA. Lewis et al., 2011 and A.V. Gourine, A. Machhada, S. Trapp, K.M. Spyer, Cardiac vagal preganglionic neurones: an update. Auton. Neurosci. 199 (2016) 24–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.autneu.2016.06.003




CRITICISM:
Reviewing more recent evidence, Paul Grossman finds RSA not "a direct measure of cardiac vagal tone" due to confounding factors. In addition, he concludes that, contrary to polyvagal claims, "there is no credible evidence that the [dorsal vagal motor nucleus] plays any role in massive bradycardia," and that it "appears to have almost no effect upon vagal heart rate responses." Grossman, Paul (2023). Fundamental challenges and likely refutations of the five basic premises of the polyvagal theory. Biological Psychology. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2023.108589.

RESPONSE FROM DR. PORGES:
These are poorly informed comments. The methodology used to quantify RSA greatly influences its sensitivity to vagal influences (see Lewis et al. 2011; Bryan & Porges, 1996). Again, when appropriate methods are used, RSA maps into the cholinergic influence. See section 7.4 of The Vagal Paradox:

'There are publications with rabbits and rats documenting fear-induced bradycardia. However, the strongest evidence comes from my laboratory in which preterm infants were tested. Preterm infants are at a phase of maturation that results in an autonomic nervous system without a functional ventral vagal circuit. This results in a propensity to react with clinically relevant massive bradycardia - as is prevalent in the perinatology literature (see Reed et al., 1999).’





CRITICISM:
In a 2021 publication, Porges states that "the theory was not proposed to be either proven or falsified." Falsifiability is a central tenet of the scientific method. Stephen W. Porges (2021-08-01), Polyvagal Theory: A biobehavioral journey to sociality. Comprehensive Psychoneuroendocrinology, vol. 7, p. 100069, doi:10.1016/j.cpnec.2021.100069, ISSN 2666-4976, PMC 9216697, PMID 35757052

RESPONSE FROM DR. PORGES:
This is taken out of context and the author presumes to know my motivation in developing the theory. It would be helpful to read the complete paragraph, which puts the phrase in context:

The theory was not proposed to be either “proven” or “falsified,” but rather to be informed by research and modified. Claims of falsification would argue that the evolved changes in the autonomic nervous system would not support a social engagement system and that socially delivered cues of safety would not calm the autonomic nervous system and consequently socialize behavior.
The theory is dependent on evolution and development to structure a hierarchical model of autonomic function inclusive of the Jacksonian principle of dissolution. This model could explain how co-regulatory social interactions are not merely ‘social’ behaviors, but neuromodulators of autonomic state via an integrated social engagement system that is capable of either supporting or disrupting homeostatic functions. Thus, aspects of social behavior can functionally support or disrupt health.
The theory uses evolution to extract a phylogenetic sequence of autonomic regulation. This sequence identifies stages during vertebrate evolution when a spinal sympathetic nervous system and the two vagal pathways emerge and become functional via maturation in mammals. It would be difficult to argue that the sequence does not occur, although it would be possible to identify antecedent similarities in most vertebrates regardless of class or group. The question is not whether there are similarities in ancestral vertebrates, but rather how these circuits have been adapted to provide a unique mammalian autonomic nervous system that is intimately intertwined with co-regulatory social behavior.


One hilarious, and a bit scary, example of this is the Grossman camp arguing how reptiles do, indeed, have an active social life similar to mammals! This they use as a strawman: Porges claims that the PVT explains how there are connections between mammal behavior and bodily functions and social interactions and e.g. facial and phonatory cues (prosody) => reptilians have an active social life too, so the whole PVT is proven false!

Anyone who sees reptilian 'social life' as similar to what mammals are engaged in must have something dark inside of him. :cool2:

Was curious about where this idea about reptiles having a social life come from. In 2021 The Secret Social Lives of Reptiles was published. Here's the amazon blurb:

Covering diverse species from garter snakes to Komodo dragons, this book delves into the evolutionary origins and fascinating details of the mysterious social lives of reptiles.

Reptiles have been too often dismissed as dull animals with tiny brains and simple, "asocial" lives. In reality, reptiles engage in a remarkable diversity of complex social behavior. They can live in families; communicate with one another while still in the egg; and hunt, feed, migrate, court, mate, nest, and hatch in groups. In The Secret Social Lives of Reptiles, J. Sean Doody, Vladimir Dinets, and Gordon M. Burghardt―three of the world's leading experts on reptiles―bring together a wave of new research with a synthesis of classic studies to produce the only authoritative look at the social behaviors of the most provocative animals on the planet.

The book covers turtles, lizards, snakes, crocodilians, and the enigmatic tuatara. Enhanced with dozens of images, it takes readers through a myriad of social interactions, tendencies, and intimacies ranging from fierce territorial battles to delicate paternal care and from promiscuous pairings to monogamous partnerships. This unique text

• explains why reptiles have been neglected as subjects of social behavior studies;
• provides numerous examples across all major reptilian groups that overturn the false paradigm of "solitary" reptiles;
• explores the sensory, genetic, physiological, life history, and other factors underlying social behavior in reptiles;
• presents the case that evolutionary "experiments" found among reptiles offer unparalleled opportunities for understanding how and why social behavior evolves in animals; and
• identifies new and developing areas of research helping to reshape our view of reptiles.

Revealing the secrets of reptilian social relationships through original quantitative research, field studies, laboratory experiments, and careful analysis of the literature, The Secret Social Lives of Reptiles elevates these fascinating animals to key players in the science of behavioral ecology.

Psychology Today ran an article about the book and interviewed one of the authors:


The Secret Social and Emotional Lives of Reptiles​

A book based on detailed research dispels myths about these fascinating animals.​

KEY POINTS​

  • Reptiles have been too often dismissed as dull animals with tiny brains and simple asocial lives. Ample research shows why this isn't so.
Reptiles are fascinating nonhuman animal (animal) beings. Many people dismiss or underestimate the depth of their social, cognitive, and emotional lives and don't realize that they're not "easy pets" and that they greatly suffer when held captive.1 I'm especially pleased that the University of Tennessee's Dr. Gordon Burghardt, a co-author of a recent encyclopedic book titled The Secret Social Lives of Reptiles, could answer a few questions about this landmark work.2,3 Here's what he had to say.

Why did you and your co-authors write The Secret Social Lives of Reptiles?
We all work on different groups of reptiles, but have been appalled by the widespread ignorance of non-avian reptile behavior and the dismissal of the view that they can have complex cognitive, emotional, and social lives. We decided that we should bring together the great amount of recent research on reptile behavior that is upending many assumptions, even by other behavioral scientists.

How does your book relate to your backgrounds and general areas of interest?

I've focused my studies of reptiles largely on snakes, turtles, iguanas, and monitor lizards. I was largely concerned with chemoreception, foraging, and antipredator behavior, usually in young animals. However, when I started doing field work in Panama, I became intrigued by the sociality of the young iguanas and my students and I sat for hours in a blind at a communal nest site and observed awesome social interactions and coordinated activity by the emerging hatchlings.

Who is your intended audience?

The book is for those who are interested in animal behavior in general as well as for a herpetological audience. It is not a trade book, but does contain lots of photos and fascinating descriptions of behavior, including extinct reptiles such as dinosaurs. We want to convince biological and social scientists, as well as the general reader, that the evidence for reptile sociality needs to be both appreciated and confronted. We also support our theme with a very lengthy reference section and encourage readers to explore these primary reports.


What are some of the topics you weave into the text and what are some of your major messages?

The book begins with an overview of social behavior, reptile evolution and relationships, and then, because reptiles are so diverse, aspects of their life history, physiology, sensory and neural features, modes of reproduction, and so on. Then we begin the chapters on social organization, courtship, communal nesting, parental care, behavioral development, and social aspects of feeding, predator avoidance, thermoregulation, and other behavioral systems.


We also review the fascinating research on cognition, social learning, emotion, and other topics. One of the main messages of the book is that there is great variation across even closely related species in social behavior, more so than in birds, mammals, or amphibians. This opens up fertile areas for comparative research that can help us understand the processes of social evolution. For example, in the large group of lizards called skinks, there are those that give live birth and also those that lay eggs. There are those that are rather short-lived and others that can live for decades. There are animals, like our American 5-lined skink, where the mother lays her eggs, guards them, and may even help them get out of their egg shells, but the offspring are soon on their own.


In some Australian skinks the babies stay with their parents, and three generations may be in the same family colony. How and why does such diversity emerge? Similar variation is seen in snakes. The cover photo of our book shows a father Gharial with babies riding on his head. In actuality, these giant crocodylians may have upwards of 100 hatchlings being protected by a single male. The species may, tragically, soon become extinct in its native India. Ours is the first book to focus on a comprehensive view of sociality in all groups of non-avian reptiles.

Are you hopeful that things will change for the better as people connect with reptiles based on some of what you've written?

I have been writing on the bias against reptiles for more than 50 years and the shifts have been minimal in many respects. However, the scientific community is now very engaged, as many of the most charismatic and large species are highly endangered, such as many sea and freshwater turtles, tortoises, crocodylians, Caribbean iguanas, and even some snakes and small lizards. The return of alligators in the Southeast US is a success story, and many people in the Southwest are now protective of their diverse rattlesnake populations. Horrible snake roundups are increasingly being targeted for elimination.


On the other hand, the slaughter of invasive reptiles, as in Florida, is troubling. The methods used would provoke serious discussion if proposed for use against invading monkeys, wild horses, deer, and so on. Here the fact that reptiles do not show anthropomorphic human-like facial expressions or scream in pain dulls our empathy with what they might be experiencing. Culling of rabbits, rodents, geese, and cats seem to arouse more concern than similar methods used for reptiles. The slaughter of reptiles, including their eggs, for food and leather, is still common globally. Collectors for the pet trade are also responsible for local and perhaps even species extinction. This is, ironically, fueled somewhat by the greatly increased popularity of reptiles as companion animals.

Is there anything else you'd like to tell readers?

Learn about the neat reptiles who live around you. They may be harder to find and observe than mammals, birds, and insects, but we evolved from their ancestors and we still share aspects of their behavior and psychology. Simple observations can be done by anyone these days and easily captured by digital cameras and phones. There are about 11,000 reptiles species, and we know virtually nothing about the majority, even in North America, but especially in the rest of the world.

References
Marc Bekoff in conversation with Gordon Burghardt.
Notes
1) For more on reptilian welfare click here.
2) Gordon M. Burghardt is Alumni Distinguished Service Professor in the departments of Psychology and Ecology & Evolutionary Biology at the University of Tennessee. He received his Ph.D. in Biopsychology from the University of Chicago. His research focus is on comparative studies of behavioral development, especially in reptiles, and play; encompassing theoretical, historical, definitional, comparative, experimental, neuroscience, and modeling questions. He is on the editorial board of numerous journals including Ethology, Journal of Comparative Psychology, American Journal of Play, Herpetological Monographs, Animals, and the International Journal of Play. He is a past president of the Animal Behavior Society and the Society for Behavioral Neuroscience and Comparative Psychology (APA Div. 6), and is the latter society’s 2020 recipient of the D. O. Hebb award for his scientific contributions. He has written, edited or co-edited 9 books, including the APA Handbook of Comparative Psychology (2017). He is author of The Genesis of Animal Play: Testing the Limits and an editor of the forthcoming Health and Welfare of Captive Reptiles (2nd edition, Springer).
3) Part of the book's description reads: Covering diverse species from garter snakes to Komodo dragons, this book delves into the evolutionary origins and fascinating details of the mysterious social lives of reptiles. Reptiles have been too often dismissed as dull animals with tiny brains and simple, "asocial" lives. In reality, reptiles engage in a remarkable diversity of complex social behavior. They can live in families; communicate with one another while still in the egg; and hunt, feed, migrate, court, mate, nest, and hatch in groups...Revealing the secrets of reptilian social relationships through original quantitative research, field studies, laboratory experiments, and careful analysis of the literature, The Secret Social Lives of Reptiles elevates these fascinating animals to key players in the science of behavioral ecology.
Bekoff, Marc. Sentient Reptiles Experience Mammalian Emotions.
_____. The Emotional Lives of Reptiles: Stress and Welfare.

Pulled up the book on google books to read the forward and this is the definition they're using for social behaviour:

social.png

Geez, under that definition rocks could be said to have a social life not to mention that the definition could include anti-social behaviour!
 
From what we have read in McGilchrist’s book, it would appear that the R hemisphere (HS) is linked to the sympathetic fright/flight system, with noradrenaline as its neurotransmitter, while the L HS controls the parasympathetic system & uses dopamine. It almost seems like it should be the other way around. However, the left HS likes the known & the familiar which makes it feel safe (parasympathetic nervous system), while the right is in correspondence with the Other/Something-beyond-the-self i.e. the unknown. It kind of makes sense then, that the R HS regulates the sympathetic (fear of the unknown), while the L HS regulates the parasympathetic nervous system. Below is a slide from our workshop discussion on The Master and his Emissary, and a webpage that summarises the differences between the HSs very well (picture is from that site).

So far from what I've read, Porges doesn't pay that much attention to LH, but he seems to think that RH regulates autonomic function and homeostasis. From what I'm understanding so far, that might mean that he thinks that RH isn't so much sympathetic, more that it might have some kind of executive control over the vagal brakes. The last is more my interpretation of what he's saying and he doesn't explicitly mention the vagal brakes in the below so this view might be updated.

THE RIGHT HEMISPHERE: THE REGULATION OF EMOTION

Right hemisphere function, evaluated via electroencephalography (EEG) or disrupted by localised damage, is related to the same cluster of behaviors that has been linked to vagal tone measure. Research demonstrates that the right hemisphere is implicated in both the expression and interpretation of emotions and in the regulation of attention. Research has also linked right hemisphere deficits with aprosody or lack of emotion expression in speech and attenuated autonomic reactivity. Several investigators have argued that the right hemisphere provided the primary control of emotion.

EEG research has been used to provide support for laterality theories of emotion. Fox and colleagues present a model of emotion expression in which positive (e.g., interest) emotions are associated with the left hemisphere and negative emotions with the right. Assymmetry of hemispheric control of negative and positive affect has also been posited by Tucker (1981). Other laterality theories focus primarily on the role of the right hemisphere in the regulation of negative emotions and fight-or-flight behaviors. The data strongly support the relation between RH EEG activity and the expression of negative emotions in infants, children and adults; however, research demonstrating the relation between LH EEG activity and the expression of positive emotions is less conclusive.

In children, RH dysfunction has been associated with attentional, social, and emotional problems. Voeller (1986) reported data on 16 children with unilateral right hemisphere lesion or dysfunction as assessed by neuro-psychological exam and/or CAT scan. Fifteen of these children were extremely distractible and inattentive, meeting the DSM-III criteria for attention deficit disorder; moreover, eight were also hyperactive. Eight children were shy and withdrawn, sharing some of the behavioral characteristics of the inhibited child described by Kagan (1994), and nine expressed atypical emotion expression (i.e., prosody, facial expression, and gesture). Most of these children made little eye contact with others, and virtually all had poor relationship with peers.

In their survey of studies with both normal and lesions subjects, Silberman and Weingartner (1986) suggested that the right hemisphere is superior for recognising emotional aspects of stimuli. They propose that the right hemisphere dominance for emotion regulation reflects a nervous system organization that gives priority to avoidance or defensive mechanisms that have a high survival value. By inference, these avoidance and defensive mechanisms require massive and immediate shifts in autonomic function.

THE RIGHT HEMISPHERE: AUTONOMIC REGULATION AND REACTIVITY

The right side of the brain also plays a special role in the regulation of emotion. Data supporting laterality theories of emotion have been based on studies of electrophysical recordings from the scalp and neurophsychological studies of dysfunction in individuals with brain damage. We propose a convergent approach by emphasizing the right brain’s regulation of peripheral autonomic activity.

Asymmetry in the control of the autonomic nervous system has been documented in the previous sections. Because peripheral organs are not symmetrical in shape or placement, it is not surprising that the neural control of the autonomic nervous system is lateralised. For example, the heart is displaced to the left, with the right vagus going to the SA node (sinoatrial node) and the left vagus going to the AV node (atrioventricular node). Other organs with dual vagal innervation are often tilted (e.g., the stomach and intestines), are located higher on one side (e.g., the kidneys), or are larger on one side (e.g., the lungs).

Emphasis on the asymmetry of autonomic organs has implications for the evolution of central regulatory systems and cortical development. In mammals, the peripheral autonomic organs and brainstem structures are similar across species. Asymmetrical neural control of autonomic processes is characteristic of mammals. However, the process of encephalization differs among mammalian species, with man possession a uniquely large cerebral cortex. Because the neural control of the vagus is ipsilateral (e.g., the left vagus originates in the left side of the brainstem), the right hemisphere – including the right cortical and subcortical structures – would promote the efficient regulation of autonomic function via the source nuclei in the brainstem. For example, neuroanatomical and electrophysiological studies demonstrate the important regulatory function of the right central nucleus of the amygdala in regulating the right nucleus ambiguous.

We propose that the functional dominance of the right side of the brain in regulating autonomic function has implications for specialization of motor and language dominance on the left side of the brain. The right-side responsibilities of regulating homeostasis and modulating physiological state in response to both internal (i.e., visceral) and external (i.e., environmental) feedback potentially enabled the control of other functions to evolve on the left side of the brain. With greater encephalization, which is characteristic of more cognitive mammalian species such as man, lateralized specialization is more observable.

A sharing of central control of voluntary and emotion-homeostatic processes would enable the individual to express complex voluntary levels of communication and movement via the left side of the brain and more intense emotion-homeostatic processes via the right side of the brain. If these processes are lateralized, they might have a degree of autonomous regulation. Of course, the central nervous system is complex and has, in many instances, both ipsilateral and contralateral communications. This provides a small percentage of individuals with central control of both language and dominant hand motor movement on the right instead of the left side of the brain. However, owing to the asymmetry of the peripheral autonomic organs and the medullary control of the autonomic nervous system, the right side of the brain is always dominant in the regulation of autonomic function and, thus, emotion.

Data from stimulation studies using left and right visual fields indicate that activation of the right cortex results in larger and more reliable autonomic responses. Additionally, studies of brain-damaged individuals have shown that right hemisphere damage or dysfunction is associated with a severe deficit in the facial , vocal, and autonomic components of the expression of emotions. Similar asymmetry of the sympathetic nervous system has been reported, with the right stellate ganglion having greater cardiovascular control than the left stellate ganglion. However, no research has focused on the assessment of cardiac vagal tone in subjects with right hemisphere disorders. Since cardiac vagal tone is neurophysiologically linked to the right hemisphere regulation of autonomic activity, it might index the individuals functional capacity to regulate autonomic function and to express emotion.
 
While looking into hibernation I come across the term 'vagotomy' and was curious that Porges doesn't seem to mention this in his studies of the vagus nerve in the same way that McGilchrist compares the operation of R & LH's through split brain and stroke patients. I think I recall him mentioning something along the lines of 'if you cut the vagus nerve......' in one of his interviews that I listened to, but the lack of comparisons with vagotomies at different levels to intact vagal nerves in Porges work is curious. Perhaps he refers to it in references, I haven't looked closely at those yet.

In any case, apparently sub diaphragmic vagotomy used to be practiced to treat peptic ulcer, but it produced the side effects of diarrhea, dumping syndrome or rapid gastric emptying, gallstones, delayed gastric emptying and weight loss. Those symptoms seem to indicate sympathetic system dominance.

There has also been a study into unilateral cervical vagotomy to modulate immune response in traumatic brain injury.

TBI induces splenic B and T cell expansion that contributes to neuroinflammation and neurodegeneration. The vagus nerve, the longest of the cranial nerves, is the predominant parasympathetic pathway allowing the central nervous system (CNS) control over peripheral organs, including regulation of inflammatory responses. One way this is accomplished is by vagus innervation of the celiac ganglion, from which the splenic nerve innervates the spleen. This splenic innervation enables modulation of the splenic immune response, including splenocyte selection, activation, and downstream signaling. Considering that the left and right vagus nerves have distinct courses, it is possible that they differentially influence the splenic immune response following a CNS injury. To test this possibility, immune cell subsets were profiled and quantified following either a left or a right unilateral vagotomy. Both unilateral vagotomies caused similar effects with respect to the percentage of B cells and in the decreased percentage of macrophages and T cells following vagotomy. We next tested the hypothesis that a left unilateral vagotomy would modulate the splenic immune response to a traumatic brain injury (TBI). Mice received a left cervical vagotomy or a sham vagotomy 3 days prior to a fluid percussion injury (FPI), a well-characterized mouse model of TBI that consistently elicits an immune and neuroimmune response. Flow cytometric analysis showed that vagotomy prior to FPI resulted in fewer CLIP+ B cells, and CD4+, CD25+, and CD8+ T cells. Vagotomy followed by FPI also resulted in an altered distribution of CD11bhigh and CD11blow macrophages. Thus, transduction of immune signals from the CNS to the periphery via the vagus nerve can be targeted to modulate the immune response following TBI.

Bilateral vagotomy has been performed in a labs studying ventricular fibrillation and defibrillation where the vagus nerve was tied in string rather than cut to reduce vagal influence on the heart and allow more sympathetic energy to influence heart rate and there are probably many more circumstances where vagotomies have been performed.
 
I recently finished this book, very very academic! but so very interesting.

I am sure some of what I will say below has been posted previously, but the sixth sense idea he has, of the internal state of viscera being also a sense that gets monitored, read and responded to by our nervous system, which much like the information obtained but he other senses do, changes our state, was fascinating. It makes absolute sense, of course, but I was astonished that I had never thought about it that way.

There's a lot that was super interesting, very informative, but the one idea that I found absolutely fascinating, in terms of intelligent design mostly, was when he would describe how certain responses from the vagus nerve would be lethal to humans, but survival techniques for reptiles, due to our metabolic and oxygen differences, a reptile can, for example, reduce the oxygen present and feign death, if a human does that, it'll probably perish!

But, we need to be able to remain immobile, as both a way to protect ourselves from danger, but also to build relationships, he compares for instance the paradox of sexual behavior in humans with the terror that freezes a human being. It's amazing how the entire system is one factor away from building and bonding with another human being, or become traumatized via the same process: freezing (which could be technically lethal) in the presence of another without dying. The three forces, the specific context principle really applies in a lot of our interactions, not just abstractly, but physiologically too!

Why would we evolve a system in our bodies, that is not within our control that could kill us? why would we possess a system that belong to another species that is so different from us?

And so, what I was really trying to mention was that he goes on to describe several systems in the ANS of human beings, some older and some newer, and he obviously takes on the evolution path when describing it. But all I kept thinking was, how a designer or engineer would create a reptile, for instance, and and find the one part of it that he could build upon to create a more complex being, and discard the rest, and repeat and so on with other beings, some mammals, some birds and other animals, and so.. when the C's said that:

A: Yes, as third density bioengineered beings, you lead the smorgasbord parade of that which surrounds you in the physical realm.

I felt that our Nervous System, and what Porges describes, is a great reflection of this bioengineering process. There's chunks from other species that sit at the base of a newer version that was assembled on top of it, that overrides it by default, but that uses it to do what it needs to do. Like what our computers do today, with so much complexity built upon binary code, or simple key commands, it's still happening, tose older systems, but it's under the hood, so to speak.

And so that's one of the things that I kept thinking when getting through this book, not sure if Porges realized that at times he was describing what I could probably describe as the signs of the bioengineering stages of humanity, in the nervous system at least.
 
There's a lot that was super interesting, very informative, but the one idea that I found absolutely fascinating, in terms of intelligent design mostly, was when he would describe how certain responses from the vagus nerve would be lethal to humans, but survival techniques for reptiles, due to our metabolic and oxygen differences, a reptile can, for example, reduce the oxygen present and feign death, if a human does that, it'll probably perish!

Yeah it is very interesting, hey? The difference in the reptile ANS as far as dorsal vagal shutdown is concerned is the reason why I started to look more into hibernation. Not that they are exactly the same, I think, but what is going on in each species might give some clues. I'm looking for papers that specifically talk about reptiles and hibernation to see if more information is revealed about what it is that is different about them in physiology and chemistry as far as the ANS goes. One of the things that I'm wondering is if there are hominids that have similarity to reptiles in their ANS. There are so many variations in ANS and biochemical actions in species that hibernate that it almost seems that it's possible that there are hominids that have a more reptilian response and can thrive and have very longs lives with extended periods of reptilian like shut down.

So digging into all that might also shed further light on these questions:

(Jones) Do bloodline 4D STS have something that is similar to an additional vagal break giving them more control over their heart rate?

A: Yes

Q: (Jones) This would mean that they are less likely to lose awareness by being in a sympathetic or dorsal vagal state when under stress?

A: More is implicated than that!

Q: (L) So it's more than just giving them control over their heart rate?

A: Yes

Q: (L) Does it mean they have like the ability to put themselves into something like suspended animation?

A: Yes

Q: (L) Did it have something to do with their ability to prolong their lives?

A: Yes

Q: (Joe) Are we talking about undergrounders here?

A: Yes

(Jones) In the sessions from October 1995, questions were asked about the MIAs and KIAs in various wars with the suggestion that they were abducted and the word 'reanimated' was used and that they're still alive.


Then from Session 13 January 2024:


The work in Transmarginal Inhibition, where an individual is flooded with stimuli that represents overwhelming threat, suggests that all previous conditioning can be undone. Deb Dana says that it is a superpower to have a nervous system that is highly sympathetically energised, but to also be able to maintain some ventral vagal connection or energy - sociality, curiosity, compassion etc. Lobaczweski says that maintaining an attitude of a naturalist studying a subject is protective when exposed to psychopaths. Victor Frankl basically says that having a meaning or reason for surviving overwhelmingly harsh experiences is protective. Were those that were selected for abduction and 'reanimation' from the wars those who went into dorsal vagal shutdown?

A: Yes


I felt that our Nervous System, and what Porges describes, is a great reflection of this bioengineering process. There's chunks from other species that sit at the base of a newer version that was assembled on top of it, that overrides it by default, but that uses it to do what it needs to do. Like what our computers do today, with so much complexity built upon binary code, or simple key commands, it's still happening, tose older systems, but it's under the hood, so to speak

I pretty much replace the word 'evolution' with 'genetic tinkering' when reading Porges. But with the idea of vagotomies added into the mix, then that might indicate that there was more than just genetic tinkering - perhaps there were other operations conducted on undergrounders.

So far in the hibernation material where it talks about reptiles, because they are ectotherms - or their body heat is dependent on the ambient temperature - they have much less energy and are opportunistic hunters, or they don't extend that much energy to find food. They're more likely to lay in wait for something to cross their path. That raises other ideas. I wonder if that's why 4DSTS cannot maintain physicality because in maintaining physicality they are more reptilian in ANS and biochemistry so they don't have the energy for maintaining physicality.

Endotherms or most mammals, require more energy, expend more energy in hunting and move around a lot more - but there are still endothermic hibernators.

Just of interest, I read a fictional tale a while ago that spoke of a group of people travelling through a mountainous area when winter hit. They basically laid up as much food as they could and wrapped themselves up in furs in a cave and slept most of the winter away waking up occasionally to eat. But because they weren't moving around so much and they were staying warm in their furs, they didn't require as much food for the duration of winter. Something like that could explain why undergrounders went underground during the last ice age as indicated in the sessions.
 
Very interesting!

It makes you think about vampires, or it makes me at least, living forever.. being cold blooded, never out during the day, and needing to feed off of the living blood of their victims. As mammals, we need to feed, so that we can keep up with our living mechanisms, like we need to be constantly generating life, using oxygen, consuming resources and transforming them into energy.. reptiles, or vampires, can't... so they have to feed off of what beings who can produce.

I also seem to remember that Adriane Raine spoke about the studies done on criminals and how their resting heart rate was significantly lower than normal human beings, it's perhaps the same ANS and vagal systems, but they're activated so differently that the being "cold blooded" moniker that we place upon them takes on a whole new meaning.

Just a few musings.
 

Trending content

Back
Top Bottom